REPORTS ON IMPLICIT BIAS 1. The Of 1: 2014] 2. State Of .

2y ago
35 Views
2 Downloads
338.79 KB
16 Pages
Last View : 23d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Adalynn Cowell
Transcription

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugustRECENT and CLASSIC IMPLICIT BIAS LITERATUREREPORTS ON IMPLICIT BIAS1. The Science of Equality, Volume 1: Addressing Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and StereotypeThreat in Education and Health Care [Godsil, Tropp, Goff & Powell, ence‐of‐Equality‐111214 web.pdf 90 page report Outlines consequences of racial bias in health care (and education), methods of countering bias,and means of improving potentially biased decision making.2. State of The Science ‐‐ Implicit Bias Review 2014, 2015, 2016 [Kirwin Institute]2014: s/2014/03/2014‐implicit‐bias.pdf2015: s/2015/05/2015‐kirwan‐implicit‐bias.pdf2016: s/2016/07/implicit‐bias‐2016.pdf Each of these extensive reports summarizes the past year’s findings on racial/ethnic implicit bias.The later reports discuss implicit bias as it relates to the fields of health, employment, education,criminal justice, and housing and to the mitigation of implicit bias.3. Nature Special Issue on Sexism in Science [Women and Bias in Science and ScientificCareers] [March ex.htmlArticles: MIND THE GENDER GAP: Despite improvements, female scientists continue to facediscrimination, unequal pay and funding disparities. [Shen, 2013] Barred from the Boardroom: The number of women in scientific research is going up — but whereacademia crosses into industry, men still rule. [McCook, 2013] What’s being female got to do with anything, ask the scientists who are starting labs and havingkids. [Ledford, Petherick, Abbott & Nordling, 2013] Most of us are biased: Let’s move beyond denial, own up to our prejudices against women andretrain our brains to overcome them [Raymond, 2013]4. Examining the Presence, Consequences, and Reduction in Implicit Bias in Health Care: ANarrative Review [Zescott, Blair & Stone, 06/1368430216642029.full.pdf “This review examines current evidence on the role that provider implicit bias may play in healthdisparities, and whether training in implicit bias can effectively reduce the biases that providersexhibit.” “Directions for future research on the presence and consequences of provider implicit bias, andbest practices for training to reduce such bias, will be discussed.”1

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugust5. Unconscious (Implicit) Bias and Health Disparities: Where Do We Go from Here? (Blair,Steiner & Havranek, ing2011/HealthDisparities.pdf “This article provides a research roadmap that spans investigations of the presence of implicit biasin health care settings, identification of mechanisms through which implicit bias operates, andinterventions that may prevent or ameliorate its effects.” “The goal of the roadmap is to expand and revitalize efforts to understand implicit bias and,ultimately, eliminate health disparities. Concrete suggestions are offered for individuals indifferent roles, including clinicians, researchers, policymakers, patients, and communitymembers.”ADMISSIONS, HIRING AND PAY IN SCIENCE & MEDICINE4. Graduating to a Pay Gap: The Earnings of Women and Men One Year after CollegeGraduation [Corbett & Hill, uation.pdf In 2009—the most recent year for which data are available—women one year out of college whowere working full time earned, on average, just 82 percent of what their male peers earned. In ananalysis by the American Association of University Women, 2/3 of this gap in pay between maleand female college graduates is attributable to education and employment factors (career sectorchoices). 1/3 of the pay gap remains unexplained. There were no significant pay differences between men and women who majored in science orhealthcare disciplines or took work in life science professions.5. How stereotypes impair women’s careers in science [Reuben, Sapienza, Zingales, act In a multi‐part experiment, employers were tasked with hiring someone to do an arithmetic task.“When the employer had no information other than candidates’ physical appearance, women wereonly half as likely to be hired as men, because they were (erroneously) perceived as less talentedfor the arithmetic task: Both men and women expected women to perform worse.” When experimenters allowed candidates to self‐report their performance, “women were chosen atequally low rates . The reason is that men are more likely to boast about their performance,whereas women tend to underestimate it.” Additionally, the employers were given an Implicit Association Test. “The initial bias in employers’beliefs correlated with implicit stereotypes about women and mathematics, as measured by the[IAT].” Also, those who had higher implicit bias were less likely to take into account that thewomen’s self‐assessment of their skill was less boastful than the men’s self‐assessment.6. Gender Differences in Salary in a Recent Cohort of Early‐Career Physician–Researchers[Jagsi et al., 816636/ “The authors observed, in this recent cohort of elite, early‐career physician–researchers, a genderdifference in salary that was not fully explained by specialty, academic rank, work hours, or evenspousal employment.”2

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugust7. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students [Moss‐Racusin, Dovidio, Brescoll,Graham & Handelsman, 2012]http://www.pnas.org/content/109/41/16474.full “In a randomized double‐blind study (n 127), science faculty from research‐intensiveuniversities rated the application materials of a student—who was randomly assigned either amale or female name—for a laboratory manager position.” “Faculty participants rated the male applicant as significantly more competent and hireable thanthe (identical) female applicant. These participants also selected a higher starting salary andoffered more career mentoring to the male applicant. The gender of the faculty participants didnot affect responses, such that female and male faculty were equally likely to exhibit bias againstthe female student.”8. Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines [Leslie,Cimpian, Meyer & Freeland, 2.full A survey of academics in 30 disciplines (including STEM and non‐STEM disciplines with both highand low female representation) designed to test the idea that the perception that a field requiresinnate talent predicts lower representation of females and racial minorities in that field. The study found: “The extent to which practitioners of a discipline believe that success depends onsheer brilliance is a strong predictor of [lower] representation [of women and African Americans]in that discipline. Our data suggest that academics who wish to diversify their fields might want todownplay talk of innate intellectual giftedness and instead highlight the importance of sustainedeffort for top‐level success in their field. We expect that such easily implementable changes wouldenhance the diversity of many academic fields.”9. Women in Academic Science: A Changing Landscape [Ceci, Ginther, Kahn & Williams, 2014]http://psi.sagepub.com/content/15/3/75.full An overview of women’s underrepresentation in math‐intensive academic fields (as opposed toless math intensive fields in which women are better represented). It finds: “Although in the past, gender discrimination was an important cause of women’sunderrepresentation in scientific academic careers, this claim has continued to be invoked after ithas ceased being a valid cause of women’s underrepresentation in math‐intensive fields.Consequently, current barriers to women’s full participation in mathematically intensive academicscience fields are rooted in pre‐college factors and the subsequent likelihood of majoring in thesefields, and future research should focus on these barriers rather than misdirecting attentiontoward historical barriers that no longer account for women’s underrepresentation in academicscience.”10. Exploring the color of glass: Letters of recommendation for female and male medicalfaculty (Trix & Psenka, 2003)http://das.sagepub.com/content/14/2/191.short A study examined over 300 letters of recommendation for medical faculty at a large Americanmedical school in the mid‐1990s.“Letters written for female applicants were found to differ systematically from those written formale applicants in the extremes of length, in the percentages lacking in basic features, in thepercentages with doubt raisers (an extended category of negative language, often associated withapparent commendation), and in frequency of mention of status terms.”3

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugust11. A Linguistic Comparison of Letters of Recommendation for Male and Female Chemistry andBiochemistry Job Applicants (Schmader, Whitehead & Wysocki, 99‐007‐9291‐4/fulltext.html “Text analysis software was used to examine 886 letters of recommendation written on behalf of235 male and 42 female applicants for either a chemistry or biochemistry faculty position at alarge U.S. research university.” “Results revealed more similarities than differences in letters written for male and femalecandidates. However, recommenders used significantly more standout adjectives to describe maleas compared to female candidates.”12. Race, ethnicity, and NIH research awards (Ginther, Schaffer, Schnell, et al, 412416/ Black applicants 10% less likely than Whites to receive NIH investigator initiated research grantsafter taking into account education, country of origin, training, previous research awards,publications, and employer.13. What Happens Before? A Field Experiment Exploring How Pay and RepresentationDifferentially Shape Bias on the Pathway Into Organizations [Milkman, Akinola, ses/apl‐0000022.pdf Results indicate that in all broad disciplines except health sciences, when making requests offaculty for the future, women and minorities, collectively, are ignored at rates that differ fromWhite male students. (While health sciences faculty were more likely to reply to resumes attachedto white male names than names associated with female gender or other racial or ethnicbackgrounds, the difference was not statistically significant.)14. Whitened Resumes: Race and Self‐Presentation in the Labor Market [Kang, DeCelles,Tilcsik & Jun, 09/0001839216639577 “Using interviews, a laboratory experiment, and a résumé audit study, we examine racialminorities’ attempts to avoid anticipated discrimination in labor markets by concealing ordownplaying racial cues in job applications, a practice known as ‘résumé whitening.’” “Results show that when targeting an employer that presents itself as valuing diversity, minorityjob applicants engage in relatively little résumé whitening and thus submit more raciallytransparent résumés.” However, an additional study showed that organizations’ diversity statements were not associatedwith reduced discrimination against un‐whitened resumes, suggesting that minorities may bemore likely to experience discrimination against ostensibly “pro‐diversity” employers.15. Males Underestimate Academic Performance of Their Female Peers in UndergraduateBiology Classrooms [Grunspan, Eddy, Brownell, et al, 2016]http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id 10.1371/journal.pone.0148405 “Among the 1,700 students surveyed in three introductory biology classes, female studentsnominated their peers equitably, while male students consistently ranked other male students asmore intelligent than their female peers. Even after controlling for class performance andoutspokenness, this bias increased over the course of the term.” This favoring of males by peers could influence student self‐confidence, and thus persistence inthis STEM discipline.4

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugust16. A lesson in bias: The relationship between implicit racial bias and performance inpedagogical contexts [Jacoby‐Senghor, Sinclair & Shelton, pii/S002210311530010X In a study of pairs of Princeton undergraduates (either white‐white or white‐black), one acting asteacher and the other as student, implicit racial bias on the part of the teacher predicted lower testperformance in the black, but not white, students. Further study suggested that the black students scored lower because of anxiety on the part of theinstructor and poorer lesson quality. New participants watched video of the cross‐race lessons totest lesson quality, and here instructors’ implicit bias again predicted test performance of theseparticipants.COMBATTING IMPLICIT BIAS1. The Effect of an Intervention to Break the Gender Bias Habit for Faculty at One Institution:A Cluster Randomized, Controlled Trial [Carnes et al., 12/The Effect of an Intervention to Break theGender.98931.pdf The authors implemented a pair‐matched, single‐blind, cluster randomized, controlled study of agender‐bias‐habit‐changing intervention at a large public university. Conclusion: “An intervention that facilitates intentional behavioral change can help faculty breakthe gender bias habit and change department climate in ways that should support the careeradvancement of women in academic medicine, science, and engineering.”2. A meta‐analytic evaluation of diversity training outcomes [Kalinoski et al., 2012]http://www.academia.edu/6058695/A meta‐analytic evaluation of diversity training outcomes “Results from 65 studies (N 8465) revealed sizable effects on affective‐based, cognitive‐based,and skill‐based outcomes as well as interesting boundary conditions for these effects on affective‐based outcomes. This study provides practical value to human resources managers and trainerswishing to implement diversity training within organizations as well as interesting theoreticaladvances for researchers.”3. How to Recognize and Address Unconscious Bias [Grewal, Ku, Girod & Valantine, 8‐1‐4614‐5693‐3 49#page‐1 “According to AAMC estimates, women make up only 35% of all medical school faculty and just19% of faculty at the rank of Full Professor. African‐Americans and those of Hispanic origin makeup only about 7% of all medical school faculty.” “We believe that until individuals and institutions address the issue of unconscious bias, facultyfrom underrepresented groups will continue to have a difficult time climbing the academic ladder.The aim of this chapter is to help the academic physician identify and understand unconsciousbias so that he or she may take steps to prevent it from negatively influencing his or her career.”5

UCSF Office of Diversity and OutreachAugust13. Do Contact and Empathy Mitigate Bias Against Gay and Lesbian People AmongHeterosexual First‐Year Medical Students? A Report From the Medical Student CHANGEStudy [Burke, Dovidio, Przeworski et al, s/articleviewer.aspx?year 2015&issue 05000&article 00030&type fulltext “This study included the 4,441 heterosexual first‐year medical students who participated in thebaseline survey of the Medical Student Cognitive Habits and Growth Evaluation Study, whichemployed a stratified random sample of 49 U.S. medical schools in fall 2010.” “Nearly half of respondents with complete data on both bias measures expressed at least someexplicit bias, and most (81.51%) exhibited at least some implicit bias against gay and lesbianindividuals. “ “Both amount and favorability of contact predicted positive implicit and explicit attitudes. Bothcognitive and emotional empathy predicted positive explicit attitudes, but not implicit attitudes.”14. The mixed impact of medical school on medical students’ implicit and explicit weight bias[Phelan, Puhl, Burke et al. du.12770/full “On average, implicit weight bias decreased and explicit [or conscious] bias increased duringmedical school, over a period of time in which implicit weight bias in the general public increasedand explicit bias remained stable.” To address this issue, “medical schools may [be able to] reduce students’ weight biases byincreasing positive contact between students and patients with obesity, eliminatingunprofessional role modelling by faculty members and residents, and altering curricula focused ontreating difficult patients.”15. An Analysis of Implicit Bias in Medical Education [Wells, Motzkus, Cashman et al, 2016]http://escholarship.umassmed.edu/ssp/239/ A qualitative analysis of an effort to teach implicit bias concepts to first‐year students at theUniversity of Massachusetts Medical School16. Reducing Implicit Gender Leadership Bias in Academic Medicine With an EducationalIntervention. [Girod, Fassiotto, Grewal et al, 2016]http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/268260686

UCSF Office of Diversity and Outreach August“Analysis of a standardized, 20‐minute educational intervention to reduce gender bias amongacademic health center faculty.”“Results indicated that the intervention significantly changed all faculty members' perceptions ofbias (P .05 across all eight measures). Although, as expected, explicit biases did not changefollowing the intervention, the intervention did have a small but significant positive effect on theimplicit biases surrounding women and leadership of all participants regardless of age or gender(P .008).”17. A “Scientific Diversity” Intervention to Reduce Gender Bias in a Sample of Life Scientists[Moss‐Racusin, van der Toorn, Dovidio et al, ort The evaluation of an intervention to reduce gender bias among undergraduate science educators. “Evidence emerged indicating the efficacy of the “Scientific Diversity” workshop, such thatparticipants were more aware of gender bias, expressed less gender bias, and were more willingto engage in actions to reduce gender bias 2 weeks after participating in the interventioncompared with 2 weeks before the intervention.”18. Constructed Criteria: Redefining Merit to Justify Discrimination (Uhlmann & Cohen, 2005)http://pss.sagepub.com/content/16/6/474.short In a series of studies, researchers showed that when hiring criteria were flexible, equally qualifiedfemales were less likely to be perceived as fit candidates for traditionally male jobs and converselyrate equally qualified male candidates lower for traditionally female jobs. Additionally, when raters were forced to decide in advance what criteria would be used to judgecandidates, women were no less likely to be selected for a traditionally male job.19. Reducing racial bias among health care providers: lessons from social‐cognitive psychology(Burgess, Van Ryn, Dovidio, et al, 06‐007‐0160‐1/fulltext.html An evidenced‐based framework for interventions to combat unintentional bias among health careproviders, drawing on social cognitive psychology.20. Non‐conscious bias in medical decision making: what can be done to reduce it? (Stone &Moskowitz, 1365‐2923.2011.04026.x/full “When activated, implicit negative attitudes and stereotypes shape how medical professionalsevaluate and interact with minority group patients.” “Cultural competence training

RECENT and CLASSIC IMPLICIT BIAS LITERATURE REPORTS ON IMPLICIT BIAS 1. The Science of Equality, Volume 1: Addressing Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat in Education and Health Care [Godsil, Tropp, Goff & Powell, 2014] . State of The Science ‐‐ Implicit Bias Review 2014 .File Size: 338KB

Related Documents:

employees about implicit bias (Meyer, 2018), and philosophers worry that implicit bias poses epistemic threats to philosophy (e.g., Saul, 2013a, 2013b; Peters, forthcoming). Nonetheless, some are skeptical about the existence of implicit bias or the efficacy of corporate

Displaying Implicit Bias Implicit bias affects more than our cognitive decision-making. Research has shown that body language is far more affected by implicit bias than verbal statements - but that most people cue in and respond more to nonverbal than to verbal cues. Particularly from people in power. (Dovidio, 2002)

found to measure the transformation of our minds. In the case of Implicit Bias research, Harvard University has sponsored "Project Implicit" which has developed a video-game-like test anyone can take online to measure their implicit bias. OPTIONS: [1] As teacher or preacher, you take the test before and after using this workbook; [2] Have

State of the science: Implicit bias in review. The Kirwan Institute. Stollaman, J. (2016). Whoops I didn’t mean to say that: Implicit bias and micro-aggressions. The Winter Institute, Oxford, MS. The Joint Commission. (2016). Implicit bias in health care, 23. The Joint Commi

describe earlier published methods of creating interpolating implicit surfaces. 2.1 Implicit Surfaces An implicit surface is defined by an implicit function, a continuous scalar-valued function over the domainR3. The implicit surface of such a function is the locus of points at which the function takes on the value zero.

Implicit associations do not necessarily align with our declared beliefs. We generally hold implicit biases that favor our own in group. Implicit biases have real world affects on our behaviors. Implicit biases are malleable, therefore can be unlearned. Kirwan Institute

Implicit attitudes and beliefs adapt to situations: A decade of research on the malleability of implicit prejudice, stereotypes, and the self-concept. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 47: 233-279. Kirwan Institute. State of the Science: Implicit Bias Review 2015. Kirwan Institute. State of

10 Annual Book of ASTM Standards, Vol 02.02, in the Related Material section (gray pages). 11 Available from Standardization Documents, Order Desk, Bldg. 4, Section D, 700 Robbins Ave., Philadelphia, PA 19111-5094, Attn: NPODS. TABLE 1 Chemical Composition Limits NOTE 1—When single units are shown, these indicate the maximum amounts permitted. NOTE 2—Analysis shall be made for the elements .