Dynamics Of Multiple Signalling Systems: Animal .

2y ago
25 Views
2 Downloads
2.76 MB
9 Pages
Last View : 10d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Tia Newell
Transcription

ReviewDynamics of multiple signallingsystems: animal communication in aworld in fluxJakob Bro-JørgensenMammalian Behaviour & Evolution Group, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, University of Liverpool, Leahurst Campus, Neston,CH64 7TE, UKThe ubiquity of multiple signalling is a long-standingpuzzle in the study of animal communication: given thecosts of producing and receiving signals, why use morethan a single cue? Focusing on sexually selected signals, Iargue that dynamic variation in selection pressures canoften explain why multiple signals coexist. In contrast toearlier research, which has taken a largely static view ofthe world, new insights highlight how fluctuations inecological and social environments, as well as non-equilibrium dynamics intrinsic to coevolutionary systems, canmaintain both multiple redundant and non-redundantsignals. Future challenges will include identifying thecircumstances under which environmental fluctuationslead to multiple signalling, and the consequences of suchfluctuations for speciation in multiple-signalling species.The ubiquity of multiple signallingDuring sexual and agonistic signalling, animals often useseveral cues to convey a message (see Glossary). Thecomposite nature of a signal can be obvious, as in the strutdisplay of the male greater sage grouse Centrocercus urophasianus, where mates are attracted by coordinated wingand tail movements combined with popping vocalizations[1] (Figure 1). However, signals that are usually regardedas single traits often comprise multiple components aswell. For example, each patch in the colour pattern of abird can be composed of several pigments conveying moreor less independent information [2], and in the sexualdisplay of the wolf spider Schizocosa stridulans, high speedcameras and laser vibrometry have revealed two distinctseismic components, produced simultaneously by theabdomen and pedipalp [3]. Signal components relying oninfra- or ultrasound [4], infrared radiation [5] and ultraviolet reflectance [6] are also easily overlooked as theycannot be detected by the human senses.But why engage in multi-component signalling insteadof concentrating on a single cue? The question is intriguinggiven that signalling is often associated with considerablecosts from time and energy loss as well as predation anddisease risk [7]. For instance, in the wolf spiders, seismicsignalling compromises the immune function of the signaller [8] and, when added to a visual display, increases therisk of predation for the signaller [9] and the receiver [10].Until recently, adaptive explanations have mostly concen-trated on static scenarios, where selection pressures areassumed to be consistent over time (Table 1a). Here,benefits of multiple signals can arise from overcomingconstraints during either signal production or reception(Boxes 1 and 2). However, it is becoming clear that dynamicselection can also explain why more than a single signal isused to convey a message (Table 1b). Focusing on sexuallyselected signals, I review how fluctuating ecological andsocial environments, as well as oscillations inherent incoevolutionary processes between signaller and receiver,can lead to multiple signalling. The review reveals thatdynamic selection might be a more widespread explanationGlossaryCrossover: when reaction norms cross so that alternative genotypes aresuperior in different environments.Cue: an informative trait, which might or might not have been selected as asignal.Fisherian runaway process: sexual selection due to positive feedback betweenan arbitrary heritable (male) trait and a corresponding heritable (female)preference; arises from increased mating success of offspring bearing the traits(‘sexy sons’), and genetic linkage between the preferred trait and thepreference (e.g. in sandflies Lutzomyia longipalpis, preferred males produceattractive sons without any apparent good-genes benefits [62]).Genotype-by-environment interaction (GEI): occurs when environmentalchange has a different effect on different genotypes. Non-parallel reactionnorms reveal GEI, with crossing reaction norms characterising strong GEI.Signal reliability can be undermined by both strong and weak GEI [13,14].Good-genes models: sexual selection models based on a heritable (female)preference for a heritable (male) trait that reflects high genetic quality (i.e.condition acquisition ability).Lek paradox: why does female preference for a specific male indicator ofindirect benefits persist when such a preference is predicted to erode thegenetic variance underlying the indicator and, as a consequence, the benefit ofthe preference?Multimodal signal: a multiple signal with at least two components in differentmodalities.Multiple (multicomponent or complex) signal: a composite signal thatcomprises two or more components, each with signal properties.Rare male effect: negative frequency-dependent selection promoting rare(male) phenotypes as a result of (female) mate preferences.Reaction norm:: a function that describes the response of a single genotype toa gradient in the environment; usually visualized as a line connecting thephenotypic expression of a genotype in two environments.Redundant signal: signal that repeats information already present in anothersignal. A signal can be redundant to another without the converse being thecase; nevertheless, two signals are often described as ‘redundant’ when theleast informative only repeats information in the other.Sexually antagonistic coevolution: coevolution owing to adaptation andcounteradaptation in sexual conflict over the optimum value of a fitnessrelated trait.Signal: a structure or action of an organism (the signaller) that is selected forits effects on the behaviour of another organism (the receiver) via its sensorynervous system in a fashion that is adaptive to the signaller and (usually) alsoto the receiver.Corresponding author: Bro-Jørgensen, J. (bro@liv.ac.uk)2920169-5347/ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.11.003 Available online 18 December 2009

ReviewTrends in Ecology and EvolutionVol.25 No.5Figure 1. Examples of organisms using multiple signalling systems. (a) The strut display of the male greater sage-grouse includes both visual and vocal elements [1]; (b) thenesting success of the male lark bunting is indicated by multi-facetted coloration and other morphological traits [18]; (c) the male wolf spider uses seismic as well as visualcomponents in courtship [10]; (d) the male guppy attracts females by intricate colour patterns [48]; (e) the eland bull Tragelaphus oryx broadcasts fighting ability bothvisually and by knee-clicking [85]; and (f) the male sagebrush lizard uses posture, headbob displays as well as chemical cues in signalling to conspecifics [80]. Photoscourtesy of Neil Losin (a); Alexis Chaine (b); Eileen A. Hebets (c); Anna Price (d); and Ahrash Bissell (f); photo (e) taken by the author.for multiple signalling than previously appreciated. Clarifying how dynamic selection operates on multiple signalstherefore offers an exciting new focus for studies in animalcommunication, with fundamental implications for understanding the maintenance of genetic variance in sexuallyselected signals, as well as speciation.Why do environmental fluctuations matter for signalselection?Animals are often exposed to drastic changes in both theirecological and social environment, including fluctuationsin resource abundance, predation pressure, disease transmission risk, habitat structure, and inter- and intraspecificcompetition. The consequences of environmental variability have been a key subject in other areas of evolutionarybiology for decades; however, the implications for sexuallyselected signalling have only come into focus more recently[11]. Although a single signal can remain reliable in afluctuating environment if it is sufficiently flexible to trackchanges in the message to be conveyed (Figure 2a,b), recentstudies have identified several ways in which single signalscan become unrepresentative of signaller quality followingenvironmental changes. Here, I suggest that temporal andspatial variability in the environment can often explainwhy multiple sexually selected signals coexist (‘fluctuatingenvironments’ hypothesis). In the following sections, I willoutline three scenarios in which multiple signalling can beadaptive in response to environmental fluctuations.Scenario 1: Static signals, but fluctuating signallerqualitySexually selected signals encompass both agonistic signalsto sexual rivals, and sexual signals to mates. Agonisticsignals convey information about fighting ability whereassexual signals convey information about mate quality (Box3). Environmental shifts can have profound effects on boththese signaller qualities [12], and in such cases, signalexpression needs to be flexible in order to reflect thechanges. However, whereas some signals remain flexiblethrough life (e.g. many behavioural traits), others areinnate or fixed during early development (e.g. manymorphological traits). Fixed signals become particularlymisleading when environmental changes have differenteffects on the quality of different genotypes (i.e. when genotype-by-environment interaction (GEI) occurs [13,14]).When environmental change thus compromises the correlation between a fixed signal and signaller quality, a newsignal can evolve from a cue which is a better indicator ofquality in the new environment (Figure 2c).As an example of how environmental change can render afixed signal unreliable, consider the horns of male bovids,which Darwin suggested might be partly selected as anintersexual signal of quality to females [15]. In the sheepOvis aries, male horn length has been found to indicate highlifetime reproductive success only in stable, benign climates;in contrast, under severe conditions, where survival mattersmore than breeding success, horn length becomes negativelyrelated to lifetime reproductive success [16]. Horn lengththus becomes an unreliable indicator of overall mate quality.In such cases, where a static signal is primarily determinedby condition during a particular developmental stage anddoes not reflect signaller quality across all contexts encountered later in life, selection can favour additional signals toreflect current condition. For example, when assessingmates, female crickets Gryllus campestris take into accountnot only male call frequency, which reflects exoskeletal size293

ReviewTrends in Ecology and Evolution Vol.25 No.5Table 1. Evolutionary hypotheses and proximate functions of multiple signalling systemsHypothesis(a) Static selection regimesMultiple messagesEmergent messageBackupAlerting signalReceiver psychologySensory overloadDescriptionRedundancy ininformationencoded(isolated vs.combinedcomponents) 1Redundancy ininformationperceived(isolated entsperceived 2ExampleEach component reflectsdistinct informationInformation resides in thecombination of signalcomponentsBackups compensate forcoding errorsOne component is an‘attention grabber’Multiple cues enhancediscriminability or learningManipulation of receiversby exploitingreduced ability to processmultiple stimuliA resp. B vs. (A B)A resp. B vs. (A B)Independence[85]A resp. B vs. CA resp. B vs. CEmergence[86] A resp. A vs. A A resp. A vs. AEquivalence[72]A resp. 0 vs. A0 resp. 0 vs. AModulation[73]A resp. A vs. Aa resp. a vs. AEnhancement[74]A resp. 0 vs. AA resp. 0 vs. ntContext-dependentContext-dependent[48](b) Dynamic selection regimesFluctuations in ecologicalFluctuating environmentsor social conditionsaffect signal informationcontent, transmission orreceiver interestsDiverging interests of theSexually antagonisticsexes over signalcoevolutionhonesty result innon-equilibriumcoexistenceof multiple signalsNegative frequency-dependentRare male effectselection arisingfrom the ‘rare male’effect maintains multiplesexual signals1The letters A, B and C indicate distinct messages, with small letters indicating part of the information content; 0 denotes no information; ‘resp.’, respectively. 2 Modified fromRef. [61].and thus condition during juvenile development, but alsochirp rate which indicates current condition [17].Is the reliability of some signal types more susceptible toenvironmental fluctuations than others? When receiversassess signals of fighting ability or non-genetic (‘direct’)benefits provided by mates, they are typically interested insignaller condition over a shorter timeframe than whenassessing signals of heritable genetic (‘indirect’) benefits,which rather depend on an individual’s lifetime performance. Environmental fluctuations might therefore have aparticularly strong effect on the reliability of the first twotypes of signals. For example, in the multiple-signallinglark bunting Calamospiza melanocorys, drastic fluctuations have been discovered in how well individual signalsreflect the direct benefits provided by males [18]. In thissocially monogamous songbird, the suite of male size andplumage traits which predicts mate quality depends on theadversity of the environment and thus varies dramaticallyfrom year to year. Corresponding to these changes in signalcontent, the signal preferences of individual females areflexible over time, tracking the traits that are currentlysuperior fitness indicators [18]. As a result, several maletraits are under positive selection in certain years only,with some of these, such as body size and various colour294patterns, even coming under negative selection in differentyears. This shows that the sexual signals of the larkbunting are maintained by fluctuating selection, althoughfurther work is needed to investigate exactly how fixedeach signal is within individual males.It should not be assumed that animals can always signaladaptively in any environment encountered. Whethereffective signals will evolve and persist depends on theevolvability of both the signal and the receiver preference,as well as on how frequently the signal provides a selectiveadvantage to the signaller. Thus in erratic environments,where some conditions are only rarely encountered, evenmultiple-signalling animals might be unable to convey amessage. For example, in the blue tit Parus caeruleus,where multiple colour signals are known to be stronglydependent on the environment [19], neither direct norindirect benefits associated with the signals have beendetected [20]. Here it is possible that none of the signalsused are adaptive in the environment studied.Scenario 2: Converging signal expression in good orpoor environmentsIn scenario 1, single signals, which were phenotypicallyfixed, were undermined by changes in the quality of the

ReviewTrends in Ecology and EvolutionVol.25 No.5Box 1. Multiple signals as equilibrium solutionsBox 2. Sensory processing of multiple stimuliSo far, multiple signalling systems have largely been consideredwithin a theoretical framework assuming static selective regimes[61,63], and empirical studies over the past decades have providedmany examples where both multiple redundant and non-redundantsignals are seen as equilibrium solutions.Focusing on signal content, the ‘multiple messages’ hypothesisproposes that the signal information value is increased most costeffectively by adding novel, non-redundant components [64].Support for this idea has been reported from a broad range oftaxonomic groups, particularly in studies of sexual and agonisticcommunication where receivers seek information on several distinctaspects of mate quality and fighting ability, respectively [26,65].According to the ‘emergent message’ hypothesis [66], theinformation relevant to receivers emerges from the interactionsbetween signal components, and animals with higher cognitiveability are indeed known to integrate separate signal elements intheir assessment of both sexual and agonistic signals. For example,when male songbirds produce a trill, they experience a tradeoffbetween the trill rate and frequency bandwidth [67]; vocalperformance therefore depends on both these traits jointly [67],and in evaluating trills, receivers of both sexes have been shown totake this into account [68,69].The ‘backup’ hypothesis maintains that multiple redundant signalscompensate for errors during information coding [70]. In general,artificial neural networks show backup signals to be most robustwhen multimodal [71]; however, unimodal and repeated signals canalso act as backups. For example, in the waggle dance of the honeybee Apis mellifera, the direction of each waggle run indicates thelocation of a food source with error, and receivers average the angleof multiple runs to improve their estimate of the location [72].Multiple signals can also improve signal efficacy rather thancontent. Attention grabbers can improve signal detection by alertingreceivers to other, more informative, signal components, as statedby the ‘alerting signal’ hypothesis [60]. For example, in the Borneanranid frog Staurois guttatus, advertisement calls cause conspecificsto orientate towards a subsequent foot-flagging display [73].Finally, the ‘receiver psychology’ hypothesis suggests thatredundant signal components enhance the accuracy and speed ofreceivers in discrimination and learning tasks [60]. For instance, indomestic fowl Gallus gallus, the reaction time of the hens to thefood-alerting signal of the cock (‘tid-bitting’) decreases when thehens are exposed to both the vocalization and the rhythmic headmovements of the cock [74]. Facilitated learning might explain whyaposematic signals are often multimodal [75]; hence the combination of visual and auditory components accelerates the speed withwhich domestic fowl learn to discriminate against unpalatable fooditems [76].New insights into the neural mechanisms behind multimodalstimulus integration are shedding new light on how neuralconstraints impact on the evolution of multiple signals. Specifically,the discovery that crossmodal integration occurs at severalcognitive levels agrees well with the diversity of evolutionaryexplanations for multiple signals (Table 1). Previously crossmodalstimulus integration in mammals was believed to be confined tohigher-level processing in the association cortex, the area whichgenerates a meaningful representation of the world and allowsabstract thought; however, evidence now shows that sensoryconvergence also occurs during early processing in the primarysensory cortices [77]. Early sensory convergence is characterizedprimarily by temporal coordination across modalities [77]. Forexample, when rhesus macaques Macaca mulatta process vocalizations from conspecifics, whether neurons in the auditory cortexrespond to combined auditory and visual components by suppression or enhancement depends on whether the stimulus onsets areseparated by more or less than 0.2 s, respectively [78]. Such simpleneural mechanisms can favour simultaneous emission of multipleredundant signals to facilitate discrimination.Another intriguing idea is that the neural suppression of incongruent multimodal stimuli might be exploited by signallers toconceal information when the interest of signaller and receiverdiverge (‘sensory overload’ hypothesis) [63]. There are severalexamples of multimodal stimuli hampering information transfer. Inmale noctuid moths Spodoptera littoralis, exposure to femalepheromones inhibits the response of the tympanic nerve toecholocation signals from bats, thereby increasing predation risk[79]. Also, in the sagebrush lizard Sceloporus graciosus, responsiveness to visual headbob displays and, in particular, to chemicalsignalling is reduced when these signals are combined [80].However, empirical evidence that signallers benefits from manipulating receivers by sensory overloading is still wantin

Dynamics of multiple signalling systems: animal communication in a world in flux Jakob Bro-Jørgensen Mammalian Behaviour & Evolution Group, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, University of Liverpool, Leahurst Campus, Neston, CH64 7TE, UK The ubiquity of multiple signalling is a long-standing puzzle

Related Documents:

Business Ready Enhancement Plan for Microsoft Dynamics Customer FAQ Updated January 2011 The Business Ready Enhancement Plan for Microsoft Dynamics is a maintenance plan available to customers of Microsoft Dynamics AX, Microsoft C5, Microsoft Dynamics CRM, Microsoft Dynamics GP, Microsoft Dynamics NAV, Microsoft Dynamics SL, Microsoft Dynamics POS, and Microsoft Dynamics RMS, and

Microsoft Dynamics 365 for Operations on-premises, Microsoft Dynamics NAV, Microsoft Dynamics GP, Microsoft Dynamics SL, Microsoft Dynamics AX 2012 or prior versions, or Microsoft Dynamics CRM 2016 or prior versions. This guide is not intended to influence the choice of Microsoft Dynamics products and services or provide technical specification.

This guide is designed to improve your understanding of how to license Microsoft Dynamics 365, Business edition. This document does not apply to Dynamics 365, Enterprise edition, Microsoft Dynamics NAV, Microsoft Dynamics GP, Microsoft Dynamics SL, Microsoft Dynamics AX 2012, or Microsoft Dynamics CRM 2016 or any other prior version.

Microsoft Dynamics Guide Dynamics GP vs. Dynamics 365 (NAV) vs. Dynamics SL . Dynamics 365 BC -1 Premium User 100/month/user (Subscription) 2000 (On-Premise) . Solomon's application became Dynamics SL. Dynamics SL is geared first and foremost for project-based businesses. This makes SL the

Microsoft Dynamics 365 for Operations on-premises, Microsoft Dynamics NAV, Microsoft Dynamics GP, Microsoft Dynamics SL, Microsoft Dynamics AX 2012 or prior versions, or Microsoft Dynamics CRM 2016 or prior versions. This guide is not intended to influence the choice of Microsoft Dynamics products and services or provide technical specification.

Engineering (Signalling) Procedure ESD-25-01 CAD & Drafting Manual for Signalling Drawings Contents Version 1.2 Date of last revision: 13 August 2010 Page 3 of 65 This document is uncontr

Engineering (Signalling) Standard ESC-21-03 Inspection and Testing of Signalling - Inspection and Testing Principles Contents Version 1.2 Date of last revision: 13 August 2010 Page 3 of 123

Fedrico Chesani Introduction to Description Logic(s) Some considerations A Description Language DL Extending DL Description Logics Description Logics and SW A simple logic: DL Concept-forming operators Sentences Semantics Entailment Sentences d 1: d 2 Concept d 1 is equivalent to concept d 2, i.e. the individuals that satisfy d 1 are precisely those that satisfy d 2 Example: PhDStudent .