DOCUMENT RESUME ED 367 581 SO 023 749 AUTHOR

2y ago
27 Views
2 Downloads
1.77 MB
110 Pages
Last View : 20d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Casen Newsome
Transcription

DOCUMENT RESUMESO 023 749ED 367 581AUTHORTITLEINSTITUTIONBezruczko, Nikolaus; Schroeder, David H.Artistic Judgment III: Artist Validation. TechnicalReport 1991-1.Johnson O'Connor Research Foundation, Inc., Chicago,IL.PUB DATENOTEPUB TYPEDec 91EDRS PRICEDESCRIPTORSMFOI/PC05 Plus Postage.Aptitude Tests; Art Education; *Artists; *CareerChoice; *Cognitive Ability; Comparative Analysis;Higher Education; Interest Inventories; ThinkingSkills; *Visual ArtsArtistic ThoughtIDENTIFIERS112p.ReportsResearch/Technical (143)ABSTRACTTwo studies compared the visual preferences,cognitive abilities, and occupational interests of artists andnonartists. Study One compared scores on an experimental battery ofartistic judgment tests for three groups: professional artists,Johnson O'Connor Research Foundation examinees in art-relatedprofessions, and Foundation examinees not in those fields. Study Twocompared the two groups of Foundation examinees on the standardFoundation battery and the interest scales of the Career OccupationalPreference System (COPS). In Study One, the artists and nonartistsdiffered significantly on all tests in the experimental battery. Onthe Barron-Welsh Art Scale (BWAS), the professional artists scoredsignificantly higher than a nonartist sample studied previously. InStudy Two, on the standard battery tests, artists scored1;ignificantly higher than nonartists in Inductive Reasoning,Structural Visualization, Paper Folding, Memory for Design,Observation, and Tweezer Dexterity. Study Two also showed thatartists and nonartists differed in their occupational interests, withartists showing significantly higher interest in artisticoccupations, and significantly less in science, business, andcomputation related fields. The Design Judgment Test, Visual DesignTest scales, and the Proportion Appraisal Consensus and .67 scaleswere shown to be valid in distinguishing artists from nonartists.Further research should be conducted into the relationship betweenartistic judgment and education and training in the visual arts.(Contains 104 references and four appendices.) ********************************Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be madefrom the original ******************************

ARTISTIC JUDGMENT III:ARTIST VALIDATIONU.& DEPARTINENT OF EDUCATIONOft* of Educatronai Research and ImprovementEDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATIONCENTER (ERIC)1Th.s document has been reproduced 11 *Cowed from the porton or otpanuationonpInatinp It0 Minor Changes hav Mtn mad* to mprovemtproduCtfon (lushlyPoints of weep omnions stated in t hi Pool,-mint do not rtscsatardy reprESnt othcmlOERI pOSMOn Or PoIrcYNikolaus Bezruczko"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THISMRIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BYandDavid H. SchroederTO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCESINFORMATION CENTER (ERICI."JOHNSON O'CONNOR RESEARCH FOUNDATION, INC.Technical Report 1991-1December 1991phrOVS1Pt :)neN7:11.17sat4c i;rJ.4111.149at

COPYRIGHT c 1992 BY JOHNSON O'CONNOR RESEARCII FOUNDATION, INCORPORATEDALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Artistic Judgment Project HI: Artist ValidationNikolaus Bezruczko and David H. SchroederABSTRACTTwo studies were conducted that compared the visual preferences, cognitiveabilities, and occupational interests of artists and nonartists. In Study One, wecompared scores on an experimental battery of artistic judgment tests for threegroups: a sample of specially recruited professional artists and a sample ofFoundation examinees divided into those in art-related occupations and those not.In Study Two, the two groups of Foundation examinees were compared on thestandard Foundation battery and the interest scales of the Career OccupationalPreference System (COPS).In Study One, the artists and nonartists differed significantly on all the tests inthe experimental battery. The differences between the professional artists and theFoundation-examinee nonartists, after differences in socioeconomic backgroundwere controlled, were greatest on the Design Judgment Test (DJT), followed bythe Visual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test (VAST). The effect for the VAST, however,was in the opposite direction from expectation. On the Barron-Welsh Art Scale(BWAS), the professional artists scored substantially higher than a nonartist samplestudied previously (Barron, 1953).In Study Two, on the standard battery tests, the examinee artists scoredsignificantly higher than the nonartists on Inductive Reasoning, StructuralVisualization, Paper Folding, Memory For Design, Observation, and TweezerDexterity. The differences ranged from .28 to .41 standard deviation units.Trends were found between artist status and tests measuring AnalyticalReasoning, Silograms, Finger Dexterity, and English Vocabulary, although theserelationships were smaller in magnitude.Study Two also showed that artists and nonartists differed in theiroccupational interests, with the artists showing significantly greater interest inartistic occupations. The magnitude of the difference on the Arts-Design scalewas .72 standard deviation units. The artists also showed significantly lessinterest in occupations related to Science-Medical-Life, Business-Finance, andComputation.In conclusion, the DJT, the two Visual Design Test scales, and the ProportionAppraisal Consensus and .57 scales were shown to be (in varying degrees) valid in4

terms of distinguishing artists from nonartists. Because the DJT and the VDTscales also show good reliability and discriminant validity, it is recommended thatthe Foundation consider using them in its standard testing battery. Furtherresearch should be conducted into the relationship between artistic judgment andeducation and training in the visual arts.

CONTENTSPageIntroductionBackground of the Artistic Judgment ProjectReview of Artistic Judgment TestingProblems With Artistic Judgment TestingThe Experimental Artistic Judgment BatteryReview of Results From the Experimental ArtisticJudgment BatteryTwo Studies of Artists .1235891113131318Study omparison of Artistic Judgment ScoresDiscriminant AnalysisComparison of Internal StructureComparison of Intertest Correlations29333535374145474747484850505052Study andard Foundation BatteryCOPS Interest ScalesDiscussionGeneral Review of the Artistic Judgment ProjectPurpose and GoalsSummary of Internal Structure and Construct ValidationValidation by Professional ArtistsOrigins of the Differences in Artistic Judgment ScoresLimitations of the InvestigationImplications for Artistic Judgment TestingiiiG5454545455575859

Implications for a Theory of Empirical Visual AestheticsIssues in the Study of Visual DesignDimensions of Designs in Visual ArtMethod of InvestigationBroader Social ImplicationsFuture Studies.6064646970.71Summary and Recommendations72References74Appendix A: Coding System for the Artistic Background Questionnairefor Foundation Examinees82Appendix B: Coding System for the Artistic Background Questionnairefor Professional Artists84Appendix C: Psychometric Analysis of Barron-Welsh Art Scale.86.89.Appendix D: Bibliographical Information for the Professional ArtistsAppendix E: Correlations Among the Artistic Judgment Tests for theProfessional Artists Not Corrected for Attenuationiv798

LIST OF TABLESPageComparison of Backgrounds of Nonartists, ExamineeArtists, and Professional Artists16Analysis of Covariance of Artistic Judgment Tests byArtist Status36Discriminant Analysis of Artistic Judgment Scales forArtists and Nonartists39Table 4Two-Way Classification Tables for Discriminant Analyses40Table 5Correlations Among the Artistic Judgment Tests for theProfessional Artists46Table 6Tests in the Standard Foundation Battery49Table 7Analysis of Covariance of Battery Tests by Artist Status51Table 3Analysis of Covariance of COPS Interest Scales by ArtistStatus53Item Statistics for the Barron-Welsh Art Scale for theProfessional Artist Sample88Table 1Table 2Table 3Table 9Table 10 Correlations Among the Artistic JudgmentTests for the Professional Artists NotCorrected for Attenuationv398

LIST OF FIGURESPageFigure 1Sample Item From Design Judgment Test19Figure 2Sample Item From Visual Designs Test21Figure 3Sample Item From Proportion Appraisal23Figure 4Item Similar to the Items on theVisual Aesthetic Sensitivity Test27Items Similar to the Items on theBarron-Welsh Art Scale30Figure 5Figure 6Artistic Background Questionnaire for the Foundation31ExamineesFigure 7Artistic Background Questionnaire for the ProlassionalArtistsvi932

ACKNOWLLDGMENTSIn this, the last in this series of technical reports concerning artistic judgment,we have attempted to reach a point of closure on the reliability and validity ofseveral artistic judgment tests. As in prior reports, we gratefully acknowledge thecooperation and assistance of the test administrators of the Washington, DC, NewOrleans, Dallas/Fort Worth, Houston, Denver, Seattle, and Philadelphia offices incollecting data for this project. The staff of the Research Department in Chicago,as well, provided a valuable service in the accurate entry and processing of thesedata, thus contributing to the validity of our results.As in the prior reports, we extend our sincere appreciation to Professors HansJ. Eysenck of the University of London and K. 0. Götz of the Düsseldorf Academyof Fine Arts. Professor Eysenck's continued interest and personal comments havehelped us appreciate the perspective of the larger academic community and relateour findings to that community. We also remain indebted to him for helping usestablish a relationship with Professor Götz, developer of the VAST. ProfessorGötz generously contributed the printed test books of the VAST that wereadministered in the testing offices.This report, however, differs from the earlier ones because of the extensivesupport, as well as active participation, that we have received from many personsoutside the Foundation, which made these studies possible. Our thanks go first tothe professional artists in the study. While artists in general are interested in thetopic of artistic judgment, convincing them of the usefulness of developing tests ofaptitude and persuading them to commit their time for testing and interviewing aredifficult. Thus we are deeply indebted to the entire sample of professional artistsfor their contribution. The commitment of these persons to their art careers andthe diversity of their backgrounds, we believe, led to the success of these studies.In addition, because of their role in both identifying professional artists toparticipate in this project and providing us with expert knowledge during thestudies, we would like to acknowledge the assistance we received from thefollowing persons: Ms. Linda Faucheux, Ms. Diana Foster, and Ms. ArleneRakoncay.Ms. Faucheux, formerly of the Johnson O'Connor Research Foundationtesting office in New Orleans, provided a key service in organizing and conductingthe testing of the artists in New Orleans. She recruited a sample of professionalartists, administered the tests and the questionnaire for this project, and collectedextensive biographical information at her own time and expense. We are indebtedto her for her personal interest and conscientious assistance because hercontribution substantially increased the usefulness of this report.vii0

Ms. Foster, formerly of the Artemesia Gallery in Chicago, contributedgenerously to the success of this report in many ways. She first noticed thesimilarity in design between .1he items in the Visual Designs Test and themovement in fine art known as Minimalism. She introduced us to the work of Ms.Lore Bert in Mainz, Germany, and Mr. Larry Puns in the United States, who arecontemporary artists, among others, who have used designs like the VisualDesigns Test items in their work, thus providing confirmation of the artistic valueof our items. She then helped us establish the criteria for professional artists bydrawing our attention to the importanc4 of juried exhibits and proceeded to recruitartists in Chicago for this study. She personally contacted many artists andlobbied on our behalf, convincing them of the importance of our studies. Finally,her assistance in the development of the artistic backg; ound questionnaires greatlyimproved the information we received from examinees and artists.Ms. Rakoncay, director of the Chicago Artist Coalition, permitted us todescribe this study and solicit artists in the Chicago Artist Guild, a newsletterdistributed in the Chicago artist community. Several artists responded to ouradvertisement and were included in the study. Her support was important ininitially stimulating the interest of artists in the study.Among the many other persons who contributed to these studies, we wouldlike to acknowledge Mr. Dan Vrabec of Vrabec Designs in Chicago, Mr. WilliamSherer of the Foundation office in New York, and Ms. Kate Pagni of the ArtInstitute of Chicago. Mr. Vrabec was an important resource to us, both byproviding the perspective of a commercial artist and helping us recruit commercialartists for the study. Mr. Sherer recruited artists in New York, thus enhancing thegeographical distribution of the artist sample. Ms. Pagni's interest in the studiesled to the participation of a number of her students.Finally, the officers of the Foundation--Mr. George Wyatt, Mr. ThomasMcAveeney, and Mr. Robert Kyle--supported this project from its beginning in1985 to the present. Together with the persons above and the continued financialsupport of the Christian A. Johnson Endeavor Foundation, they have made possiblethe studies that we report here.

INTRODUCTIONThis is the third report in a series that presents the results of a project to studyseveral tests of artistic judgment conducted by the Johnson O'Connor ResearchFoundation. In the first report, Artistic Judgment Project I: Internal-StructureAnalyses (Technical Report 1989-2), we described the internal psychometricproperties of several tests of artistic judgment that were administered to a sampleof Foundation examinees. Our goal was to determine the internal consistency ofthe items, assess their precision in measuring differences between persons, andascertain whether any of the tests appeared promising as tests of artistic judgmentaptitude.In the second report, Artistic Judgment II: Construct Validation (TechnicalReport 1990-4), we looked at data from this same sample of examinees, butanalyzed the relationships of their scores to external criteria of validity that wereboth art- and nrinart-reiated. We examined the associations of artistic judgmenttest scores w' ch: (a) the tests in the Foundation's standard aptitude battery, (b)the items or an artistic background questionnaire, (c) occupational interest scales,and (d) biographical data including college majors and years of education for theexaminees in the study.In this report, our goal is to examine the differences in test scores betweenartists and nonartists on the experimental ardstic judgment battery, thus extendingour understanding of the validity of these tests. We do so by comparing the testscores for a group of professional artists to the scores for our sample of examineesfrom the Foundation testing offices. The examinee sample was divided into laypersons ("nonartists") and persons in art-related occupations ("examinee artists").The comparison between nonartists, examinee artists, and professional artists isthe primary focus of this report and, in our opinion, provides definitive validationfor the tests in the experimental battery as measures of art-related characteristics.A secondary focus, in this report, is a comparison between artists andnonartists on tests in the standard Foundation battery and an occupational interestinventory. We do this comparing the examinee artists with the nonartists. Theintent of this comparison is to build upon the earlier comparison of artists andnonartists, enriching our understanding of the distinctiveness of artists. Theresults in this report are reported here as two studies: Study One consists of thecomparison between professional artists, examinee artists, and nonartists, whileStudy Two represents the comparisons between the examinee artists andnonartists in the Foundation sample.Before reporting the results of these two studies, we describe briefly thebackground for the artistic judgment project, including descriptions of the12

experimental artistic judgment tests and a brief review of the results of theinternal-structure and construct-validation analyses.Background of the Artistic Judgment ProjectFor many years, researchers at the Foundation and elsewhere have speculatedthat artistic judgment is associated with one or more aptitudes and related to theproduction of visual art. Over the last 60 years, the Foundation has experimentedwith several tests designed to measure artistic judgment including the McAdory ArtTest (McAdory, 1929), the Design Judgment Test (Graves, 1948), and ProportionAppraisal (Technical Report 44), but over time the Foundation became dissatisfiedwith each of them. None of these tests are currently in the Foundation's standardbattery or commercially in print, and the search for a valid and reliable test ofartistic judgment remains a priority.The early tests of artistic judgment relied on a method of testing that requiredexaminees to indicate their preference for a design from a selection of two ormcre. The McAdory Art Test (McAdory, 1929), for example, published byColumbia University Press and the first test of artistic judgment that received wideattention, presented items that consisted of four similar pictures, and the examineewas asked to select, for each item, the picture that he or she liked the best.Unlike earlier researchers (Fechner, 1865; Thorndike, 1916), McAdory selected thepictures for her test items from common magazines and specialized art sources andthen created variants for each picture by modifying its line arrangement, shape, orcolor (McAdory, 1929). Another innovation in the development of the McAdoryArt Test was her use of art experts to establish a standard for scoring theresponses to test items.The Design Judgment Test, published by The Psychological Corporation, isanother test that has been used widely and assesses preference for visual designs.The author manipulated several design characteristics in the construction of theitems, although the portion of the test that we administered, a 22-item subset,presents items that vary primarily in symmetry. An innovation in the DesignJudgment Test over earlier tests is its use of nonrepresentational designs.Proportion Appraisal, developed and used within the Foundation, wasconstructed in the 1930s to test preference for geometric designs thatsystematically vary in their shape (this preference was believed to be related togeneral artistic judgment). This emphasis on shape as an influence on preferenceis consistent with a principle in art theory, first discussed by the ancient Greeks,which states that the proportions of an artwork fundamentally influence itsattractiveness (see later section for details).2:3

Although each of these tests showed promise, each also had limitations, aswill be discussed.Review of Artistic Judgment TestingBeginning in 1985, the Foundation undertook a review of the empiricalresearch on artistic judgment that has been conducted outside the Foundation.Bezruczko (Technical Report 1988-1) found three primary approaches to the studyof artistic judgment, namely, psychophysics and mathematics, mental testing andpsychometrics, and a psychobiological approach that implements principles ofinformation theory; the approaches are summarized briefly below.Psychophysics and mathematics. The earliest empirical work in this areaappeared between 1865 and 1376 (Fechner, 1865, 1876), when Fechnerdeveloped objective methods for investigating differences between persons in theirpreference for controlled visual stimuli. While his intention was to demonstrate anempirical basis for his personally held philosophical convictions, the most influentialaspects of his studies were not his results but rather the empirical methods that hedeveloped, now referred to as psychophysics. They have become widely appliedin all areas of psychology and form the foundation for the modern science ofmental testing.Following the psychophysical tradition, Birkhoff, a prominent mathematician ofthe 20th century, formulated a mathematical treatment of artistic judgment(Birkhoff, 1932; see also Birkhoff, 1956). He speculated that aesthetic experienceconsists of perceptual phases in which, respectively, the complexity, aestheticvalue, and order of an object influence the formation of an artistic judgment.Although he constructed a set of polygons based on his model, he never tested themodel empirically, and subsequent research has not supported it.Mental testing and psychometrics. A second approach to artistic judgmentdeveloped with the rise of the mental testing movement during the early 20thcentury. Beginning in 1910, tests of artistic judgment were developed, withThorndike (1916) and McAdory (1929) conducting some of the earliest studies.Their work was followed by many other attempts to develop instruments thatmeasured artistic judgment, with the Meier Art Tests (Meier, 1928, 1942, 1963)and the Design Judgment Test (Graves 1946, 1948) the most thoroughlyresearched and widely used.Simultaneous with the development of a testing approach to artistic judgmentwas psychometric research into the primary factors that underlie a person'spreference for visual designs. The most noted research in this area was byEysenck (1940, 1941), who conducted extensive studies involving many samples.He found substantial evidence for a general preference factor ("T," for taste) that3

extends across all preference judgments and suggested that this general ractor hasa neurological origin and constitutes the basis for fundamental perceptualdifferences between persons (Eysenck, 1957). Further research yielded anotherfactor, "K," on which artists and nonartists differ in their preferences. T is acommon factor that reflects aspects of visual designs, such as order and harmony,on which artists and nonartists agree, while K is a factor that discriminatesbetween them, with artists preferring lesser complexity than nonartists.Psychobiological approach. The psychophysical/mathematical and testingapproaches were followed by a third approach in the 1960s led by Berlyne (1971,1974). Relying heavily upon psychobiological theory, Berlyne proposed a model inwhich persons differ in their preference for visual complexity because of apreferred or optimal level of arousal and show differences in their need for visualstimulation. His empirical stud:6a showed very explicitly that visual complexity is apowerful influence on preference, and he presented a neurological processing.model, implementing concepts from information theory, to explain the underlyingprocess involved in forming a visual preference. Unfortunately, his studies did notexamine systematically the differences between artists and nonartists, and thus hisstatements concerning complexity and preference have led to considerableconfusion concerning their relationships both to artistic judgment and humanbehavior in general.Summary of literature review. The research most directly applicable to themeasurement of artistic judgment is the factor analytic studies by Eysenck. Theyshowed that several factors underlie visual preference (Eysenck, 1940, 1941,1970, 1972a) and that the two most important factors are T and K.Despite decades of empirical investigation, however, research into visualpreference variables has not led to an understanding or clarification of their role inartistic judgment or their usefulness for aptitude testing. Researchers disagree onthe prevalence of a common factor for artistic judgment (Child, 1964) and do notagree on the association between complexity and artists' preference. WhileEysenck (1940, 1941, 1970, 1972a) and Brighouse (1939) found that artistsprefer less-complex designs and nonartists more-complex designs, Munsinger andKesson (1964) reported the opposite relationship, as did Barron and Welsh (1952)and others (Eisenman, 1966; Eisenman & Rappaport, 1967). The influence oforder on artist preference also is not clear. Birkhoff, in a test of the preference forpolygons, emphasized the influence of order, while Attneave (1959) and Garner(1970) discussed the influence of repetition, or redundancy. Similarly, thedifference between the preferences of artists and nonartists for symmetry isemphasized by Graves (1948) but not replicated by other researchers (Eysenck,1970; Eysenck & Castle, 1971; Gätz & Götz, 1974).4

Despite their inconsistencies, these earlier studies of artistic judgment tend tolink differences in visual preferences between artists and nonartists to severaltypes of visual designs, including: asymmetrical designs (Eysenck, 1970),polygons characterized by low levels of complexity (Eysenck, 1968, 1972a; seealso Eysenck & Castle, 1970b), and paintings characterized by order, balance, andharmony. Similarly, the inconclusiveness of research concerning a generalstatistical factor T on which artists and nonartists agree has not diminished thepractical importance of K, a factor on which they disagree. A substantial amountof evidence shows significant group differences between artists and nonartists onK, although no standardized instruments to our knowledge have used this factor tomeasure artistic judgment.The question cf interest to the Foundation--Is artistic judgment an aptitude?--isnot addressed directly in the research literature. The literature concerning themeasurement of artistic judgment tends to assume that persons differ in theirvisual preferences and that some persons have a special capacity to make artisticjudgments. Artistic judgment, however, is generally not discussed explicitly as anaptitude, although differences in artistic judgment are expected to occurindependently of training or education.Problems With Artistic Judgment TestingEmpirical studies of the preference judgments that persons make whenchoosing designs have identified several problems with using these judgments totest artistic judgment. These problems include the operational definition of thevariables that the tests are intended to measure, the methods used to validate theconstructs that underlie the tests, the psychometric properties of the items in thetests, and the aesthetic value of the designs used in the tests. These problems arediscussed briefly below.Construction of designs. The concepts on which many tests of artisticjudgment are based are vague and ambiguous, and the construction of designs forthem oftentimes appears arbitrary. The Design Judgment Test (full-length version)illustrates this problem. The author describes the construction of the designs interms of the "basic principles of order unity, dominance, balance, continuity,symmetry, proportion [and so on] . . ." (Graves, 1948) but does not provide anexplicit description or systematic explanation for how these characteristics aremanipulated in the designs. In the Design Judgment Test, this problem is notsevere because the designs are relatively simple, so that we can infer theunderlying principles through simple inspection. But other tests, such as theBarron-Welsh Art Scaie, despite decades of research, are still not adequatelyunderstood. The Barron-Welsh Art Scale was not based on explicit criteria thatwere systematically implemented in the construction of its designs, and thusresearchers have speculated about, but never resolved, a question concerning its5

underlying construct. Researchers have been inclined to characterize the constructas a contrast between preferences for simple and complex designs (Barron, 1953,1982) or for some other particular characteristic of the items. Because thisinterpretation is ad hoc, it is always open to reinterpretation and revision, thusultimately confusing a field of study already characterized by ambiguity anduncertainty. The state of affairs for the Design Judgment Test and the BarronWelsh Art Scale is true in general for the empirical study of artistic judgment.Unfortunately, when test developers have tried to use objective, systemdcmethods to develop tests of preference, using only designs with controlledcharacteristics, such as in Birkhoff's test using polygons that differ primarily intheir number of sides, the results have not, in general, been very satisfactory. Forexample, Child examined the correlates of responses to designs from Birkhoff'stest and found only a weak association with art criteria (Child, 1964). While otherresearchers have developed designs on the basis of quantitative principles(Attneave, 1957; Noll, 1966, 1972), their designs have not been assembled intotests.Criterion validity. The issue of validity, discussed above in relation to theconstruct for a test, is problematic for artistic judgment tests. Researchers havefound that expert art opinion tends not to be stable, with correlations betweenexpert artists sometimes as low as .33 (Getzels & Csikszentmihalyi, 1969).Moreover, the use of criterion groups has shown, in some cases, extremevariability in judgments across generations. In a study of the Design JudgmentTest, Eysenck (1970; Eysenck & Castle, 1971) found that the difference betweenartists and nonartists on the test reported in the 1940s to be 28 points, haddiminished twenty years later to a magnitude that was not statistically significant.An influence on validation results that has not received considerable attentionfrom researchers is the differences in preference that may exist between artisticspecialties such as contemporary versus classical art, abstract versus concrete art,or commercial versus fine a

DOCUMENT RESUME. ED 367 581 SO 023 749. AUTHOR Bezruczko, Nikolaus; Schroeder, David H. TITLE. Artistic Judgment III: Artist Validation. Technical. Report 1991-1. INSTITUTION Johnson O'Connor Research Foundation, Inc., Chicago, IL. PUB DATE Dec 91 NOTE. 112p. PUB TYPE Reports Re

Related Documents:

Chapter 7: RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot 88. About the RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot 89 Access the Inventory Groups in a Process Unit 90 Access the RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot Page of a Functional Location 91 Access the RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot Page of an Asset 95 Access the RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot Page of a Corrosion Loop 96 Access the RBI 581 ISO Risk Plot Page of a .

VAS 581 001 Proper use 5 Proper use The VAS 581 001 A/CServiceCenter (ASE 581 01 00 000) is designed for performing maintenance on vehicle air conditioning systems. The unit is designed for commercial use.

Turtle Wax MARINE WAX (581) PUNKT 1: Identifikation af stoffet/blandingen og af selskabet/virksomheden Udgivet dato 03.09.2012 1.1. Produktidentifikator Kemikaliets navn Turtle Wax MARINE WAX (581) Synonymer FG5604-361A Marine Wax Artikel nr. 581 1.2. Relevante identificerede anvendelser fo

ASTM D3307 Type I III II IX Melt Flow Rate g/10 min ASTM D3307/ISO 12086 14 5 2 42 Melting Point C ( F) ASTM D4591 305 (581) 305 (581) 305 (581) 305 (581) Specific Gravity — ASTM D792 2.15 2.15 2.15 2.15 Critical Shear Rate, 372 C (702 F) 1/sec — 50 21 12 250 Tensile

DOCUMENT RESUME. ED 428 581 FL 025 780. AUTHOR Cowie, Neil James TITLE Collaborative Journalling by Email: Using the Structure of. Cooperative Development To Become a More Reflective Teacher. PUB DATE 1997-00-00 NOTE. 14p. PUB TYPE Journal Articles (080) -- Reports Research (143) JO

DOCUMENT RESUME ED 381 581 TM 023 051 AUTHOR Baldwin, Beatrice . Numerous symptoms of school environment problems may exist including high student and faculty absenteeism, student cliques and gangs, negative talk in . Internship

DOCUMENT RESUME ED 304 581 CE 052 124 AUTHOR Carnevale, Anthony P.; Gainer, Leila J. TITLE The Learning Enterprise. INSTITUTION American Society for Training and Development, Alexandria, VA. SFONS AGENCY Employment and Training Administration (DOL), Washington, D.C. PUB

Asset Management Sector Report 1. This is a report for the House of Commons Committee on Exiting the European Union following the motion passed at the Opposition Day debate on 1 November, which called on the Government to provide the Committee with impact assessments arising from the sectoral analysis it has conducted with regards to the list of 58 sectors referred to in the answer of 26 June .