The Effects Of Pretrial Publicity On Juror Verdicts: A .

2y ago
30 Views
2 Downloads
984.14 KB
17 Pages
Last View : 27d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Cannon Runnels
Transcription

Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1999The Effects of Pretrial Publicity on Juror Verdicts:A Meta-Analytic ReviewNancy Mehrkens Steblay,1,3 Jasmina Besirevic,1 Solomon M. Fulero,2and Belia Jimenez-Lorente1The effect of pretrial publicity (PTP) on juror verdicts was examined through ameta-analysis of 44 empirical tests representing 5,755 subjects. In support of thehypothesis, subjects exposed to negative PTP were significantly more likely to judge thedefendant guilty compared to subjects exposed to less or no negative PTP. Greater effectsizes were produced in studies which included a pretrial verdict assessment, use of thepotential juror pool as subjects, multiple points of negative information included in thePTP, real PTP, crimes of murder, sexual abuse, or drugs, and greater length of timebetween PTP exposure and judgment. The effect was attenuated with student subjects,use of general rather than specific PTP information, certain types of PTP content, apost-trial predeliberation verdict, and specific types of crimes. Implications of theseresults are discussed, along with possible mechanisms that underlie the PTP effect.In legal cases that achieve community- or nationwide notoriety, the attendantpublicity is often massive. Even prior to a trial, one need only mention a criminaldefendant's last name (Sheppard, Manson, Simpson, McVeigh, Kaczynski) ornickname (Boston Strangler, Hillside Strangler, Son of Sam, Unabomber) to trigger recognition and recall of facts and innuendo about the case gleaned frommedia sources. In such cases, two constitutional guarantees—the First Amendment right of freedom of the press and the Sixth Amendment right to a fairtrial—often collide.The question of whether pretrial publicity (PTP) about criminal cases has aneffect on the ultimate outcome of the resulting trial may appear to be a 20th centuryphenomenon, but it actually has a long history. As long ago as 1846, a New Yorklegal writer (Note, 1846) stated that:1Department of Psychology, Augsburg College, Minneapolis, Minnesota.Department of Psychology, Sinclair College, Dayton, Ohio (e-mail: sfulero@sinclair.edu).Correspondence should be directed to Dr. Nancy Steblay, Department of Psychology, Augsburg College,2211 Riverside Avenue, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55454 (e-mail: steblay@augsburg.edu).232190147-7307/99/0400-0219 16.00/1 C 1999 American Psychology-Law Society/Division 41 of the American Psychological Association

220Steblay, Besirevic, Fulero, and Jimenez-LorenteOurs is the greatest newspaper reading population in the world; there is not a man amongus fit to serve as a juror, who does not read the newspapers. Every great and startlingcrime is paraded in their columns, with all the minuteness of detail that an eagercompetitor for public favor can supply. Hence, the usual question, which has now becomealmost a necessary form in empaneling a jury, "have you formed or expressed an opinion?"is virtually equivalent to the inquiry, "do you read the newspapers?" . . . . In the case ofa particularly audacious crime that has been widely discussed it is utterly impossible thatany man of common intelligence, and not wholly secluded from society, should be found,who had not formed an opinion (p. 198).In the media-saturated modern world and with the rise of multiple formsof communication such as the Internet, this assertion is likely even more accurate.Of course, the crux of the long-standing controversy about PTP effects is notmerely that a potential juror possesses information about the case. Instead, thequestion is whether the PTP will affect that juror's consideration of evidence andultimate decision in the case he or she is selected to hear. Currently, courts attempt to gauge the likelihood that PTP has violated a defendant's rights and toapply appropriate judicial remedies to ameliorate a problem. For example, a highrate of community awareness of potentially prejudicial PTP and its possible linkto prejudgment of defendent guilt is often the foundation for a change of venuemotion.Over the past 30 years, empirical studies have also attempted to determinewhether PTP affects juror verdicts. As other scholars have noted, laboratory studieshave been criticized for lack of external validity, while naturalistic studies of realjurors and cases have not been able to link directly PTP prejudgment effects withfinal juror verdicts (Carroll, Kerr, Alfini, Weaver, MacCoun, & Feldman, 1986;Fulero, 1987; Linz & Penrod, 1992). In addition, researchers have operationalizedand presented PTP in different ways (for example, in writing or on video) andmeasured its effects using multiple methods (for example, through verdicts beforeor after deliberation). From a research perspective, these varied approaches areessential as a means to test the parameters of the PTP effect. However, the lackof unity complicates the task of a reader who hopes to ascertain the relationshipbetween PTP and juror opinion.What is common among the studies is the hypothesis that jurors exposed tonegative pretrial publicity will produce higher percentages of guilty verdicts thanjurors exposed to more neutral PTP. At first blush, the results appear less thancohesive. A quick tally of research outcomes indicates that of 44 empirical tests ofthe PTP hypothesis, 23 supported the hypothesis, 20 reported no signficant difference at the traditional .05 level, and 1 produced a significant result in the oppositedirection. It is likely that this apparent fragmentation of the research base is whathas driven some researchers to opposing conclusions about the PTP effect (Carrollet al., 1986; Fulero, 1987).Fortunately, the review technique of meta-analysis provides the means to define, from a broader data base than the individual study, the presence or absenceand magnitude of a PTP effect. The purpose of meta-analysis is to identify anyunderlying pattern across studies, damping the noise of extraneous error components among individual studies. In particular, statistical limitations of small samplesmay be overcome; the combination of many data sets and the examination of effect

Meta-Analysis of PTP Effects221size indicators provide the tools to uncover effects otherwise masked by low power.Additionally, meta-analysis may provide information regarding variables, methodological or theoretical, which moderate the examined effects. The usefulness of metaanalysis in general is thoroughly reviewed elsewhere (Rosenthal, 1991) andexamples of meta-analysis applied to fundamental psycholegal questions are available (e.g., Steblay, 1992, on weapon focus, and Steblay, 1997, on lineup instructionbias). In the case of pretrial publicity effects, the most salient question for a metaanalysis is quite specific—Does PTP have an effect on juror judgment of a defendant? Beyond this, the data set may offer clues to the underlying mechanics ofsuch an effect and the conditions under which it is most likely to occur. Thus, itmight be possible to inhibit the negative impact of PTP in our criminal justice system. In this light, a meta-analytic review of the PTP effect may be a means toinform and guide legal policy and decision making.Thus, the goals of this meta-analysis are (a) to determine the effect, if any,of negative pretrial publicity upon jurors' judgments of defendant guilt, (b) if suchan effect occurs, to identify the conditions under which the PTP effect is mostlikely to occur, (c) to examine the impact of methodological variations in the research, and (d) to identify areas for future research.METHODSampleA computer search of the CD-Rom database PsycLIT provided an initialsample of studies relevant to the hypothesis. E-mail and an electronic listserve(PSYLAW-L) were used to contact researchers in an attempt to locate additionalworks, published or unpublished. In order to be included in the sample, the studymust have provided a statistical test of the relationship between pretrial publicityand individual subjects' judgments about guilt or innocence of a defendant. Thereview includes both survey investigations and jury simulation experiments inwhich negative information about the defendant and crime was given to the subject prior to a trial. Operational definitions of PTP and control conditions weredecided by the authors of the individual studies; thus type and levels of PTPpresented to subjects varied across studies. The dependent measure (guilt or innocence of the defendent) was measured as a dichotomous choice in some studiesand through a Likert-type scale (with endpoints of guilty and not guilty) in otherstudies. In three studies in which "not sure" was an option for respondents(Deluca, 1979; Simon, 1966; Simon & Eimermann, 1971), those responses werenot included in the analyses.Twenty-three articles were located (18 published and 5 unpublished), providing44 tests of the hypothesis. The sample included work published between 1966 and1997, representing 5,755 subjects. Sample sizes ranged from 37 to 535, with a meanof 130.79 (see Appendix).

222Steblay, Besirevic, Fulero, and Jimenez-LorenteStudy CharacteristicsMethodological and theoretical variables were coded as part of the data set.Methodological variables included researcher, year of publication, source (published,convention paper, unpublished), number of hypothesis tests per study, sample size,type of study (survey of residents, experimental simulation), information providedto control group (no information, limited information about crime, limited information about both defendant and crime), origin of stimulus trial (real vs. fabricated),and origin of the PTP (real vs. fabricated). Variables of theoretical import includedtype of PTP information provided (confession, arrest record, etc.), PTP medium(newspaper, video, combined), PTP focus (general vs. specific to defendant), daysof delay between PTP and juror decision, time of verdict (pretrial, posttrial beforegroup deliberation, posttrial after group deliberation), and type of crime portrayedin the PTP. The dependent variable was the subject's judgment of the culpability ofthe defendant. Three of the authors independently read and coded each article. Thecoders then came together to check for oversights in the coding process. The variables coded were derived directly from the articles, with minimal interpretation necessary. Multiple coders were employed simply to assure that available informationwas recorded correctly. Thus ultimate agreement among coders was 100%.StatisticsFollowing the work of Rosenthal (1991), the Pearson correlation coefficientr was used as the measure of effect size. The mean effect size for a group of studiesis referred to in subsequent discussion simply as r. A meta-analytic Z (Zma) wascalculated by combining Z-scores of individual tests of the hypothesis using theStouffer method (Rosenthal, 1991). This method produces an overall probabilitylevel associated with the observed pattern of results. A fail-safe N (Nfs) was calculated to estimate the number of additional tests averaging null results that wouldbe needed in order to bring the significance level attained through the meta-analysisto a value larger than .05.In individual studies for which precise r and Z calculations were not possible,conservative estimates were used. When authors reported no significant effect, r 0.0 and z 0.0 were used. When authors reported a significant effect with animprecise p value, the most conservative estimate was employed (e.g., p .05 became a one-tailed z 1.65).RESULTSOverall Test of the PTP HypothesisForty-four tests of the hypothesis were first examined in an attempt to determine the overall status of the PTP effect, i.e., the hypothesis that negative PTPwill increase judgments of defendant guilt. Judgment of guilt was compared betweenconditions in which subjects had experienced different levels of PTP. In support of

Meta-Analysis of PTP Effects223Table 1. Stem-and-Leaf Displayof Effect he hypothesis, the meta-analytic Z (Z m a ) is 13.13, p .0001, with subjects exposedto negative PTP more likely to judge the defendant guilty than subjects exposedto less or no negative PTP The associated fail-safe N for this group of studies is2,755, indicating that this large number of nonsupportive studies would be necessaryto change the conclusions to nonsupport of the hypothesis. Twenty-eight of the testsallowed calculation of percentage of guilty verdicts within conditions: In the PTPcondition, the percentage of guilty verdicts averaged 59%; in the control conditions,the mean was 45%.A mean effect size of r .16 was obtained (positive r and Zma values indicategreater levels of guilty verdicts). Three studies (Hoiberg & Stires, 1973; Moran &Cutler, 1991; Ogloff & Vidmer, 1994) did not provide necessary data for precisecalculations for all statistics; thus, as noted above, conservative estimates were used.With these estimates eliminated from the pool, the overall figures change minimally,Zma 12.21, r .16.An additional consideration is the potential for nonindependence of multipletests from the same laboratory. It is possible that similarity in temporal and procedural factors may produce a heightened correlation of results. To provide a corrected weighting for multiple tests, a single r and z value for each of 26 independentinvestigations was calculated, averaging across multiple tests within each study.Again, this had minimal impact on the overall Z value, Zma 11.95, and produceda slightly higher r of .20.As the stem-and-leaf plot (see Table 1) illustrates, the distribution of effectsizes around the mean of .16 is relatively normal (95% CI: -.13 to .46; 99% CI:-.23 to .55). However, there are clearly outliers in the data set and variability (arange in rs from -.22 to .76 and confidence intervals which include zero.). A test

Table 2. Guilty Verdicts— Effect Sizes for Pretrial Publicity VersusControl GroupModerator variableResearch hTime of verdictPretrialPosttrial, predeliberationPosttrial, postdeliberationDelay between PTP and verdictZero (immediate)1 day1 weekMore than 1 weekPTP contentMultiple componentsMotorcycle gang memberLow-status jobHeinous crimeConfessionPrior recordInformation re: related crimeWeak inadmissible defendant statementRace of defendantNegative characterMistaken acquittal related caseStrong inadmissible evidence"Victims' rights agenda"Control conditionNo information about crime/defendantInformation about crime onlyLimited information (crime/defendant)Dissimilar caseStimulus materialsReal PTPFictitious PTPPTP specificitySpecific to defendantGeneralPTP MediumNewspaperVideoBothType of crimeDrug useSexual abuseMurderMultiple crimesRapeEmbezzlementArmed robberyDisorderly conductPersonal injuryData sourcePublishedUnpublishedaEffectsize 1.62.61.30.05Zma 1.65 is significant at .05 level.When Ns do not add to 44, this indicates that assigning data to separateconditions within some studies was not possible.b

Meta-Analysis of PTP Effects225of homogeneity reveals substantial variability as well, x2(43) 409.38, p .05.Subsequent analyses attempted to explore moderator variables which might determine conditions under which the PTP effect is most pronounced or constrained.In these analyses the pool of all 44 tests of the hypothesis was considered.Moderator VariablesResults of moderator variable analysis are reported in Table 2. Using the meanr of .16 as an anchor point, we discuss subsets of studies with notably elevated orattenuated effects below.Survey Versus Simulation DesignThe largest effect size, r .39, was recorded for a series of studies by twoteams of researchers (Costantini & King, 1980/81; Moran & Cutler, 1991) in whicha survey strategy was used to test the relationship between PTP and juror decisionmaking in legal venues for real cases. These researchers contacted jury-eligible residents of communities in which crimes had occurred, assessed each subject's degreeof knowledge on a specific case, and compared those subjects with high PTP recollection to those with low information. Perceived culpability of the defendant correlated significantly with PTP knowledge. The 95% CI around r of .39 is -.45 to 1.23; 99% CI: -.71 to 1.49. This interval calculation is affected strongly by thesmall sample size of 5; the range of actual effect sizes is from .22 to .66. These 5tests of the hypothesis generated a significant Zma of 10.16, Nfs 186.The remaining 39 tests of the hypothesis employed experimental simulationdesigns in which subjects were exposed to controlled amounts of PTP and thenasked to render a verdict. These tests generated an lesser effect size of r .14(95% CI: -.17 to .45; 99% CI: -.27 to .55), but still a significant Zma 10.30,Nfs 1488.Given the greater effect strength and consistency of the survey samples, additional analyses attempted to determine if salient components of the survey technique (e.g., subject pool, time of verdict) strengthened the PTP effect in the surveyresearch. Table 3 documents this exploration of factors, listing effect sizes for keyTable 3. Survey Data Set Impact on Primary ModeratorsEffect size rModeratorDelay: 7 daysSubject pool: residentsTime of verdict: pretrialPTP source: real PTPControl group: limited informationMedium: both video and printPTP content: multiple componentsCompletedata setSimulationstudies only (N).36.30.29.28.25.23.22.28 (2).25 (10).24 (12).19 (6).21 (16).09 (5).19 (24)

226Steblay, Besirevic, Fulero, and Jimenez-Lorentemoderator variables with and without survey tests included in the analyses. Effectsizes were somewhat larger in survey studies for each factor: the longer delay between exposure to PTP and decision, a subject pool of community residents, thetiming of the verdict (pretrial), real PTP including multiple components of negativeinformation in both video and print, and the strategy of employing a low-information control group. These features also appear to generate effects in simulationstudies, but to a lesser extent. The exception was the subgroup of studies whichused combined print and video PTP. In the full data set, r was .23; for simulationstudies alone, the effect size was only .09.SubjectsA greater effect size was obtained when the subject pool drew from potentialjurors (community residents), r .30, than when studies employed students (r .08). As shown in Table 3, those studies which employed both residents and studentsfell midway between the other groups (r .16).Time SequenceTwo issues of time are relevant as moderators. First, the point at which averdict was requested was investigated. An effect size of .28 with a Zma of 12.47was produced in the 17 tests which requested an individual verdict from subjectsprior to viewing any trial information. Subjects who provided verdicts after viewinga trial, either before or after deliberation, produced significant results also, but withsmaller effect sizes (r .10 and r .15, respectively). A second issue is the lengthof delay between exposure to PTP and subsequent decision about the defendant.A longer delay (more than one week) between PTP exposure and juror judgmentwas associated with the larger effect size for this moderator (r .36).PTP ContentAs can be seen in Table 2, there is substantial variability in effect sizes acrossdifferent operational definitions of PTP content. However, there was a somewhatstronger effect genera

Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 23, No. 2, 1999 The Effects of Pretrial Publicity on Juror Verdicts: A Meta-Analytic Review Nancy Mehrkens Steblay,1,3 Jasmina Besirevic,1 Solomon M. Fulero,2 and Belia Jimenez-Lorente1 The effect of pretrial

Related Documents:

es to specified pretrial release conditions and pretrial practices. The findings of the legal review related to specified pretrial release conditions and pretrial practices are provided below. 'Blanket' Pretrial Release Condition 'Blanket' pretrial release condition is a term used to describe one or more conditions imposed upon

May 02, 2018 · D. Program Evaluation ͟The organization has provided a description of the framework for how each program will be evaluated. The framework should include all the elements below: ͟The evaluation methods are cost-effective for the organization ͟Quantitative and qualitative data is being collected (at Basics tier, data collection must have begun)

Silat is a combative art of self-defense and survival rooted from Matay archipelago. It was traced at thé early of Langkasuka Kingdom (2nd century CE) till thé reign of Melaka (Malaysia) Sultanate era (13th century). Silat has now evolved to become part of social culture and tradition with thé appearance of a fine physical and spiritual .

On an exceptional basis, Member States may request UNESCO to provide thé candidates with access to thé platform so they can complète thé form by themselves. Thèse requests must be addressed to esd rize unesco. or by 15 A ril 2021 UNESCO will provide thé nomineewith accessto thé platform via their émail address.

̶The leading indicator of employee engagement is based on the quality of the relationship between employee and supervisor Empower your managers! ̶Help them understand the impact on the organization ̶Share important changes, plan options, tasks, and deadlines ̶Provide key messages and talking points ̶Prepare them to answer employee questions

Dr. Sunita Bharatwal** Dr. Pawan Garga*** Abstract Customer satisfaction is derived from thè functionalities and values, a product or Service can provide. The current study aims to segregate thè dimensions of ordine Service quality and gather insights on its impact on web shopping. The trends of purchases have

The Virginia Pretrial Services Training and Resource Manual is intended to serve as an instruction manual for new pretrial officers as well as an informative reference for existing staff. The manual contains all of the critical information and resources pretrial officers need to perform their duties as they relate to Pretrial Services.

7.Advanced Engineering Mathematics - Chandrika Prasad & Reena Garg 8.Engineering Mathematics - I, Reena Garg . MAULANA ABUL KALAM AZAD UNIVERSITY OF TECHNOLOGY B.Sc. IN NAUTICAL SCIENCE SEMESTER – I BNS 103 NAUTICAL PHYSICS 80 Hrs 1 Heat and Thermodynamics: 15 hrs Heat Transfer Mechanism: Conduction, Convection and Radiation, Expansion of solids, liquids and gases, application to liquid .