A Meta-analysis Of Cognitive Change With Haloperidol In .

3y ago
21 Views
2 Downloads
298.68 KB
14 Pages
Last View : 1m ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Azalea Piercy
Transcription

Schizophrenia Research 89 (2007) 211 – 224www.elsevier.com/locate/schresA meta-analysis of cognitive change with haloperidol inclinical trials of atypical antipsychotics: Dose effectsand comparison to practice effectsNeil D. Woodward a,⁎, Scot E. Purdon b , Herbert Y. Meltzer c , David H. Zald aaDepartment of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, 301 Wilson Hall, 111-21st Ave. S., Nashville, TN, 37203, USAbDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, CanadacDepartment of Psychiatry, Vanderbilt University Medical School, Nashville, TN, USAReceived 4 May 2006; received in revised form 24 August 2006; accepted 28 August 2006Available online 23 October 2006AbstractProspective, double-blind, randomized trials comparing atypical antipsychotic drugs (APDs) to typical APDs, such ashaloperidol, indicate that atypical APDs provide a modest benefit to cognitive function in schizophrenia. However, the validity ofthis inference has been contested by suggestions that the cognitive improvements observed with atypical APDs reflect practiceeffects associated with repeated testing on the same neuropsychological instruments, or an avoidance of a deleterious effect ofhaloperidol on cognitive function that might be dose related. These alternate hypotheses were assessed by meta-analyses that 1)examined the relationship between cognitive change and dose of haloperidol within the control arms of prospective atypical vs.typical APD clinical trials; and 2) compared the magnitude of change observed within the haloperidol arms of these studies toestimated practice effects for several commonly used neuropsychological measures. The results indicate that overall cognitiveperformance improves while on haloperidol. Studies that used a low dose of haloperidol (b 10 mg) did not yield larger effect sizesfor overall cognitive function or specific neuropsychological measures than studies that used a high dose (N 10 mg), although dosesgreater than 24 mg appear to have deleterious effects. For two of the six neuropsychological tests examined (digit symbolsubstitution and verbal fluency) the magnitude of change observed was significantly less than practice effects. The results indicatethat although haloperidol may cause deleterious effects at very high doses, or in specific cognitive domains, these effects are notlikely to explain the broader range of cognitive improvements observed with atypical APDs. 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.Keywords: Schizophrenia; Neuropsychology; Haloperidol; Practice effects; Meta-analysis1. IntroductionCognitive impairment is common in schizophreniaand is recognized as an important determinant of functional outcome (Green et al., 2000; Green, 1996;⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: 1 615 322 5584; fax: 1 615 343 8449.E-mail address: neil.woodward@vanderbilt.edu (N.D. Woodward).0920-9964/ - see front matter 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.021Heinrichs and Zakzanis, 1998). Findings from numerousdouble-blind, random assignment clinical trials indicatethat atypical antipsychotic drugs (APDs) such as clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and quetiapine, improvecognitive function, compared to typical APDs such ashaloperidol, in schizophrenia (Bilder et al., 2002; Purdonet al., 2001, 2000; Lee et al., 1999; Harvey et al., 2003;Keefe et al., 2004). The improvements are observed in

212N.D. Woodward et al. / Schizophrenia Research 89 (2007) 211–224global cognitive function and in specific cognitivedomains such as learning and processing speed; althoughthe mean difference between atypical and typical APDstends to be rather small with effect sizes ranging from 0.2to 0.4 (Woodward et al., 2005). The benefits to cognitionassociated with atypical APDs are often attributed to thenovel pharmacological actions of these agents whichinclude, but are not limited to, their ability to enhancedopamine and acetylcholine release in the prefrontalcortex (PFC) (Ichikawa et al., 2002; Meltzer, 2004;Meltzer and McGurk, 1999).In prospective atypical vs. typical APD trials inwhich patients undergo neuropsychological evaluationat baseline and again at least once after random assignment to treatment, any differences between treatments in the degree of change observed over time areattributed to an enhancement of cognition with atypicalAPDs and not the effect of repeated exposure to theneuropsychological test materials and/or assessmentenvironment (i.e. practice effects) (McCaffrey et al.,2000). However, this conclusion relies on the assumption that haloperidol and associated increased use ofadjunctive anticholinergic medications to control emergent extra-pyramidal symptoms (EPS) have benigncognitive profiles that do not interfere with normalcognitive processes, including practice effects. This assumption has been challenged by speculation that thecognitive benefits associated with atypical APDs may,in part, result from an avoidance of the deleteriouseffects associated with typical APDs rather than a novelenhancement of cognition (Carpenter and Gold, 2002;Tandon et al., 1999; Blyler and Gold, 2000; Kasper andResinger, 2003; Purdon et al., 2003; Harvey and Keefe,2001). Specifically, it has been argued that typicalAPDs, haloperidol in particular, may exert a subtlenegative effect on cognition that impedes normal practice effects (Harvey and Keefe, 2001; Carpenter andGold, 2002). Moreover, if the negative effect of haloperidol is dose related, then the relatively high doses ofhaloperidol used in some demonstrations of atypicalAPD cognitive advantages may have further biased theresults (Carpenter and Gold, 2002; Geddes et al., 2000;Harvey and Keefe, 2001; Gold, 2004). In the context ofcorporate-sponsored clinical trails designed to determine the efficacy of atypical APDs, this may lead toinappropriate dosing of haloperidol in the control armand a misattribution of the avoidance of a deleteriouseffect of high doses of haloperidol to a novel effect ofthe atypical APD under investigation.Unfortunately, evidence to support or refute thesealternate hypotheses remains sparse or largely circumstantial (Meltzer and Sumiyoshi, 2003). The hypothesisthat typical APDs, primarily haloperidol, interfere withpractice effects is reasonable given that haloperidol canelicit significant EPS and may also result in selectiveimpairments in processing speed, motor skill, andprocedural learning as a consequence of D2 receptorblockade in the dorsal striatum (Legangneux et al., 2000;Ramaekers et al., 1999; Kapur et al., 2000; Purdon et al.,2003, 2002; Bedard et al., 2000, 1996; Kumari et al.,1997). Anticholinergic medications used to treat EPS,which are typically prescribed with greater frequency intypical APD control arms of clinical trials, can alsoimpair cognitive processes related to learning andmemory (Zachariah et al., 2002; McGurk et al., 2004;Gelenberg et al., 1989; Spohn and Strauss, 1989).Interestingly, a recent meta-analysis of both longitudinaland cross-sectional studies found that, contrary toexpectations, typical APDs actually improve overallcognitive function in schizophrenia compared to notreatment or placebo (Mishara and Goldberg, 2004).Although the previous meta-analysis of cognitive changeto typical APDs did not find a relationship between doseor concealment of study drug and change in overallcognitive function, the extent to which the findings fromthe Mishara and Goldberg (2004) meta-analysis extendto double-blind, random assignment, atypical vs. typicalAPD clinical trials is uncertain given that more than halfof the 34 studies included in the meta-analysis werenaturalistic single sample, open label studies. In ourprevious meta-analysis of atypical APDs, we observedthat the open label trials produced larger effect sizes forsome neuropsychological domains indicating that resultsfrom open label trials do not always generalize to doubleblind and/or random assignment trials. It must also benoted that in the Mishara and Goldberg meta-analysisonly slightly more than a third of the studies examinedcognitive change with haloperidol exclusively, leading toa potential confounding of effects between haloperidoland other typical APDs. Moreover, only one clinical trialcomparing an atypical to a typical APD was included.This is an important qualifier in that clinical trialscomparing atypical to typical APDs may suffer fromunique biases such as a possible tendency towards usinghigher doses of haloperidol, prophylactic anticholinergictreatment, and corporate sponsorship which are unlikelyto impact studies examining the effect of various typicalAPDs on cognition relative to placebo, or unmedicatedpatient groups. Finally, the previous meta-analysis byMishara and Goldberg (2004) did not examine potentially selective deleterious effects of high doses of typicalAPDs on specific neuropsychological tests or cognitivedomains such as processing speed and motor skill that,putatively, may be adversely affected by haloperidol.

N.D. Woodward et al. / Schizophrenia Research 89 (2007) 211–224The proposition that atypical cognitive efficacy trialsare biased towards identifying positive effects becausethey utilized haloperidol doses that were too high wasinitially based primarily on the findings of a small studyof risperidone vs. low dose haloperidol that failed toconfirm the cognitive benefits of risperidone identified inearlier, largely corporate sponsored clinical trials thatused higher doses of haloperidol (Green et al., 2002).Subsequent trials using lower doses of haloperidol andlarger sample sizes confirmed the benefits of risperidoneand other atypical APDs to overall cognitive function;although the differences tend to be smaller than earlierstudies (Keefe et al., 2006; Harvey et al., 2005).Nonetheless, the relatively modest benefits of olanzapineand risperidone to cognition, compared to earlier trials,reported for two recent trials was attributed largely to thelow doses of haloperidol used relative to earlier trials(Keefe et al., 2004, 2006). However, speculation thathaloperidol has a subtle deleterious effect on cognitionthat interferes with normal practice effects in atypicalAPD clinical trials has relied on anecdotal reports without empirical substance (Meltzer and Sumiyoshi, 2003).To the contrary, slight improvement in cognition hasbeen observed with haloperidol in several clinical trials(Purdon et al., 2000; Keefe et al., 2004), although theimprovements have not been systematically examinedand compared across clinical trials. Also, since placebotrials are understandably rare, there has been no estimation of the magnitude of the expected practice effectsthat haloperidol is deemed to modulate. The metaanalyses described below constitute a systematic quantitative review of the literature pertaining to haloperidoleffects on cognitive skill reported from clinical trials ofatypical APDs. The analyses will evaluate the hypotheses that haloperidol offers no cognitive benefit, thathigh dose haloperidol confers less improvement or evendetriments to cognitive skill that are not apparent withlow dose haloperidol, and that benefits observed withhaloperidol will not equal the benefits anticipated frompractice effects alone.2. MethodsProspective investigations of atypical APD efficacywere reviewed to extract the magnitude of the cognitivechanges reported in haloperidol arms. The potentialcontribution of haloperidol dose was examined bystratification and comparison of low dose to high dosestudies, and by analysis of correlations between haloperidol dose and the magnitude of the cognitivechanges. The potential mitigation of practice effectsfrom haloperidol was examined by comparison of the213change observed in the haloperidol arms to practiceeffects estimated from healthy control samples.2.1. Analysis one: cognitive change with haloperidol2.1.1. Literature search, inclusion criteria, and codingof study characteristicsThe literature search, inclusion criteria, and coding ofstudy characteristics are identical to those used in ourprior meta-analysis of cognitive change with atypicalAPDs (Woodward et al., 2005). However, the databaseof controlled studies used in the prior report was updatedto include studies published or ‘in press’ as of July 2005to capture two new large scale, double-blind randomassignment studies comparing olanzapine and risperidone to haloperidol (Keefe et al., 2006; Harvey et al.,2005). Sixteen studies reporting data from fourteenindependent clinical trials of atypical APDs met criteriafor inclusion and are listed in Table 1. One study, Leeet al. (1999), randomized subjects to a variety of typicalAPDs, not exclusively haloperidol; however, this studywas still included in the meta-analysis since haloperidolwas the most common typical APD subjects received.Two studies included in the prior meta-analysis wereexcluded from this analysis (Smith et al., 2001; Velliganet al., 2003). One study, Smith et al. (2001), was notincluded because within group means and SDs forcognitive measures were not reported at the end of thedouble-blind phase of the trial, could not be derivedfrom the reported statistics, and could not be obtainedfrom the author. The second study, Velligan et al.Table 1Haloperidol studies included in meta-analysisStudyRe-test intervalBilder et al. (2002)Buchanan et al. (1994)Green et al. (2002)Green et al. (1997)aHarvey et al. (2005)Keefe et al. (2004)Keefe et al. (2006)Kern et al. (1999)aLee et al. (1999)McGurk et al. (1997)aLiu et al. (2000)Potkin et al. (2001)Purdon et al. (2000)Purdon et al. (2001)Rosenheck et al. (2003)Velligan et al. (2002)14104412128464125.5624b52b24abStudies reported data from same clinical trial.Endpoint data used instead of first retest.

214N.D. Woodward et al. / Schizophrenia Research 89 (2007) 211–224(2003), was not included because test–retest means forcognitive measures were not reported or could not bederived from the reported statistics, and also because itwas unclear how many subjects were randomized tocontinue receiving haloperidol during the trial. Studieswere coded for author and year of publication, numberof follow-up cognitive assessments and test–retestintervals, use of alternate forms for tests of learningand memory, mean or median dose of haloperidol, andthe mean age, education, IQ, illness duration, and age atonset of patients included in the haloperidol group.2.1.2. Neuropsychological tests and calculation ofeffect sizesEffect sizes were calculated for overall cognitivefunction by calculating a Global Cognitive Index andselected neuropsychological tests in order to examinewidespread and selective effects of haloperidol oncognition, respectively. Several studies reported astandardized cognitive summary score and in thesecases the change in this score was used as the effect sizefor the Global Cognitive Index. For studies that did notreport a standardized cognitive summary score, theGlobal Cognitive Index was calculated by averagingeffect sizes across all neuropsychological tests includedin the study. Mean effect sizes were also calculated forthe specific neuropsychological tests listed in Table 2. Intwo cases, verbal list learning and Digit SymbolSubstitution, highly similar tests were combined into asingle measure. The choice of specific tests to include inthis review was based on their frequency of use inclinical trials (the test was used in at least four studiescomprising a total of at least 100 subjects), putativesensitivity to potentially deleterious effect of haloperidol(i.e. processing speed and motor skill), their establishedrelevance to functional outcome in schizophrenia, andtheir correspondence with candidate tests for inclusionin the MATRICS cognitive battery (Green et al., 2000;Green, 1996; Nuechterlein et al., 2004).The standardized mean difference (SMD) methodwas used to calculate effect sizes. Effect sizes werecalculated within groups by subtracting the baselinescore from the retest score and dividing the difference bythe pooled baseline and retest standard deviations (SDs)(Dunlop et al., 1996). If baseline and retest means andSDs were not reported, then the effect size was estimated from the mean change score divided by its SD, ifavailable, or from the t or F statistics when changescores were also not reported (Shadish and Haddock,1994). For studies that included more than one followup assessment, only data from the first retest was usedwhenever possible in order to maximize the sample sizefrom each study, and guard against a selection biasanticipated from a more frequent and early withdrawalTable 2Neuropsychological tests included in meta-analysisDomainTestAbbreviationDependent variableTrailmaking AContinuous performance testTMACPTTime to completed-primeProcessing speedDigit symbol/modalities testTrailmaking BDSSTTMBNumber of items completed in time limitTime to completeExecutive functionWisconsin Card Sorting TestWCSTPerseverative or % perseverative errorsVerbal learningCalifornia/Rey/Crawford/Bushcke/Verbal Learning TestsVLLiTotal number of words recalled over learning trialsDelayed verbal recallCalifornia/Rey/Crawford/Bushcke Verbal Learning TestsVLLdNumber of words recalled from list after delay periodVerbal fluencyControlled Oral Word Association TestCategory Instance Generation TestCOWACIGTNumber of words generatedNumber of words generatedFTTGPBNumber of taps-averaged across both handsTime to complete-averaged across both handsAttentionMotor skillFinger Tapping/Oscillation TestGrooved Pegboard Test

N.D. Woodward et al. / Schizophrenia Research 89 (2007) 211–224of patients assigned to haloperidol arms. Fourteenstudies included only one re-test or reported data fromthe first re-test that could be used to calculate effect sizes[data were provided upon request from study authors fortwo trials (Keefe et al., 2006; Purdon et al., 2000)]. Trialendpoint data were used to calculate effect sizes for theremaining two studies (see Table 1). It is unlikely thatthese two trials produced exaggerated effect sizes giventhat the LOCF data used to calculate effect sizes for one,Purdon et al. (2001), was based on a total of 11 subjects,of which only 3 completed a third and final assessment,and inspection of the figures included in the originalreport of the other study, Rosenheck et al. (2003),indicated that patients within the haloperidol arm did notdemonstrate any additional changes in cognition beyondthe first re-test.A weighted average effect size and 95% confidenceinterval (CI) was then calculated for the Global CognitiveIndex and the selected neuropsychological tests bycombining effect sizes across studies according to thefixed effects model (Hedges and Vevea, 1998). The fixedeffects model was used because we were interested indrawing conclusions about the specific set of publishedatypical vs. typical APD clinical trials, not the moregeneral issue of cognitive change with typical APDs. Toassess the degree of variance in effect sizes across studies,a measure of effect size heterogeneity, the Q statistic, wasalso calculated for each neuropsychological test (Hedges,1994). The critical alpha for the Q statistic was set at .10(Petitti, 2001). When the assumption of homogeneity wasrejected the effect sizes were combined using the randomeffects model (Hedges and Vevea, 1998). The weightedmean effect size, 95% CI, along with the total number ofstudies and subjects at follow-up for each neuropsychological test are reported. In addition, publication bias, thetendency for studies reporting significant effects to bemore likely published than studies that did not findsignificant effects, was assessed by plotting Global Cognitive Index effect sizes against sample size (i.e. FunnelPlot). An absence of publication bias is usually assumedif the shape of the plot approximates the shape of ainverted funnel with greater spread of effect sizes aroundthe mean for studies with smaller sample sizes than thosewith larger sample sizes (Begg, 1994).2.1.3. Haloperidol dose and moderat

cognitive profiles that do not interfere with normal cognitive processes, including practice effects. This as-sumption has been challenged by speculation that the cognitive benefits associated with atypical APDs may, in part, result from an avoidance of the deleterious effects associated with typical APDs rather than a novel

Related Documents:

Meta-analysis using Stata Prepare data for meta-analysis Declaring a meta-analysis model Declaring a meta-analysis model In addition to effect sizes and their standard errors, one of the main components of your MA declaration is that of an MA model. metaoffers three models: random-effects (random), the

Part III: Meta-Analysis of Experimental Effects and Other Dichotomous Comparisons 241 6. Treatment Effects: Experimental Artifacts and Their Impact 243 7. Meta-Analysis Methods for d Values 273 8. Technical Questions in Meta-Analysis of d Values 335 Part IV: General Issues in Meta-Analysis 391 9. General Technical Issues in Meta-Analysis 393 10.

of study designs. These approaches include meta-study, meta-summary, grounded formal theory, meta-ethnography, and qualitative meta-synthesis. In this workshop, we will focus on qualitative meta-synthesis by presenting a six-step approach for conducting this type of systematic review and sharing our procedures and results from our own studies.

Meta-Analysis “Meta-analysis is a statistical technique for combining the results of independent, but similar, studies to obtain an overall estimate of treatment effect.” “While all meta-analyses are based on systematic review of literature, not all systematic reviews necessarily include meta-

Indeed, the computations required for the most basic meta-analytic work are so trivial that in my own meta-analytic work of the last 30 years or so, I have never felt the need to use a software package that "does meta-analysis." Good software for meta-analytic procedures can, of course, be a great time saver. However, a drawback to the .

abilities. The authors concluded that chess instruction is a way to develop higher-order thinking skills useful for math problem solving. Most research on meta-cognitive abilities and chess training in primary schools concentrates on the transfer of specific chess skills to specific meta-cognitive abilities used in the math domain. Less is known on

ification, using spatially rotated meta-atoms, two proof-of-concept kaleidoscopic meta-plexers, using spatially rotated meta-atoms, are designed using the feature of asymmetric CP reflections. The first meta-multiplexer exhibits low RCS in the spin-up state and nondiffracting propagation

given topic (also called “systematic literature review”). Meta-analysis: a specific statistical strategy for assembling the results of several studies into a single estimate(4). Although many people use the term meta-analysis interchangeably with systematic review, strictly speaking a meta-analysis is an optional component of a .