Implicit Theories Of Personality And Attributions Of .

3y ago
26 Views
1 Downloads
591.18 KB
17 Pages
Last View : 8d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Axel Lin
Transcription

Child Development, xxxx 2013, Volume 00, Number 0, Pages 1–17Implicit Theories of Personality and Attributions of Hostile Intent:A Meta-Analysis, an Experiment, and a Longitudinal InterventionDavid S. YeagerAdriana S. MiuUniversity of Texas at AustinEmory UniversityJoseph Powers and Carol S. DweckStanford UniversityPast research has shown that hostile schemas and adverse experiences predict the hostile attributional bias.This research proposes that seemingly nonhostile beliefs (implicit theories about the malleability of personality) may also play a role in shaping it. Study 1 meta-analytically summarized 11 original tests of this hypothesis (N 1,659), and showed that among diverse adolescents aged 13–16 a fixed or entity theory aboutpersonality traits predicted greater hostile attributional biases, which mediated an effect on aggressive desires.Study 2 experimentally changed adolescents’ implicit theories toward a malleable or incremental view andshowed a reduction in hostile intent attributions. Study 3 delivered an incremental theory intervention thatreduced hostile intent attributions and aggressive desires over an 8-month period.The hostile attributional bias is the tendency to interpret ambiguous provocation as intentional, that is,to view others’ negative actions toward you as purposeful and hostile when their intention is unclear(Dodge, 1980, 2006). For example, imagine an adolescent who is walking down the hallway in hisschool. A peer runs down the hallway, knockinghim over and spilling his books on the floor, causing other peers to laugh (Dodge, 2006, p. 791). Wasthis done on purpose or was it an accident? Whatwould the student do in response?Over a hundred studies have demonstrated thata hostile attributional bias is a predictor of thedesire to enact reactive aggression. This has beenshown in response to hypothetical scenarios, laboratory provocations, and provocations in real-worldsettings (Dodge, 2006; Dodge, Coie, & Lynam,2006). Research has also shown that changing thishostile bias toward more of a benign attribution ofintent can reduce children’s and adolescents’ reactive aggression (e.g., Hudley & Graham, 1993; for aSupport for this research was provided by the Spencer Foundation and the Thrive Foundation for Youth. The authors aregrateful to the students and teachers who participated in thisresearch and to April House, April Scott, Kerry Morrissey, Shannon Morrissey, Ahmad Saleh, Shannon Brady, Cary Catching,Liz Chamberlain, Brian Spitzer, Kali Trzesniewski, Michelle Harris, Jessica Reed, and Rebecca Johnson for their assistance conducting this research.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed toDavid Yeager, 108 E. Dean Keeton Stop A8000, Austin, TX 787121043. Electronic mail may be sent to yeager@psy.utexas.edu.meta-analysis, see Wilson & Lipsey, 2006). Combined, these constitute some of the most robust,generative, and important findings in all of developmental psychology. Indeed, this research has provided leverage for understanding and addressingpeer aggression, one of the most pressing problemsfacing today’s youth.But where does the hostile attributional biascome from? Dodge (2006) has theorized that pasthostile experiences, such as abuse or long-termexposure to violent contexts, can lead to hostileschemas that produce a heightened vigilance topeer hostility and therefore promote hostile intentattributions. Although such hostile experiences andschemas are undoubtedly influential, we proposethat a hostile attributional bias can also emergefrom more seemingly nonhostile sources, such asan implicit theory about whether people’s traits arefixed and unchangeable. More specifically, we propose that adolescents who hold an entity theory ofpersonality—the idea that people’s traits cannotchange (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995; Erdley &Dweck, 1993; Yeager, Trzesniewski, Tirri, Nokelainen, & Dweck, 2011)—will be more likely to attribute ambiguous provocations to a peer’s hostileintent and, because of this, to express a greater 2013 The AuthorsChild Development 2013 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2013/xxxx-xxxxDOI: 10.1111/cdev.12062

2Yeager, Miu, Powers, and Dweckdesire for aggressive revenge. This is because thosewith more of an entity theory see people’s goodand bad behaviors as emanating from enduringtraits rather than being a product of circumstances(Erdley & Dweck, 1993; see also Chiu, Hong, &Dweck, 1997; Levy & Dweck, 1999), and so theymay then be more likely to conclude that a peerwho upset them is a bad person who upset themon purpose. If this were true, then one method forreducing attributions of hostile intent might be tochange adolescents’ implicit theories toward moreof a malleable or incremental view of personalitytraits.To test these proposals, we conducted the present research. Study 1 was a meta-analysis of 11 original correlational tests of the hypothesis that anentity theory would predict greater attributions ofhostile intent following both imagined and experienced provocations. Study 1 also explored whetherthese increased hostile attributions might statistically mediate the effect of an entity theory on thedesire to respond aggressively. Study 2 addressedthe causal role of implicit theories. It experimentallychanged adolescents’ theories toward an incremental view and observed differences in attributions ofhostile intent. Study 3 extended this by conductinga longitudinal experiment that measured the effectsof an incremental theory intervention on hostileattributions and a desire for vengeance over an 8month period.Schemas That Shape Hostile Attributional BiasesAs noted, Dodge (2006) has argued that attributions of hostile intent are a function of hostile schemas that result in part from negative life eventsand that hostile attributional tendencies mediate theimpact of these negative schemas on behavior. Forinstance, research has documented that socializationfrom parents who, themselves, have hostile attributional tendencies can predict children’s increasedhostile attributional styles (MacBrayer, Milich, &Hundley, 2003), as can frequent experiences of peervictimization (Yeung & Leadbeater, 2007), or theexperience of abuse during childhood (Dodge,Bates, & Pettit, 1990; see Dodge, 2006, for a review).Relatedly, other research has shown that hostile“schemas”—such as the chronic accessibility of hostile thoughts—can predict attributions of hostileintent and because of this predict aggressive desiresor behavior (Burks, Laird, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates,1999; Dodge, Laird, Lochman, & Zelli, 2002; seeDodge, 2006, for a review). Overall, this is important evidence that hostile experiences and schemasmight produce hostile attributional tendencies.However, how complete is this explanation of theorigins of the hostile attributional bias?Implicit TheoriesWe propose that other, seemingly nonhostileschemas—ones that may or may not arise from hostile experiences and are distinct from overall negative views about the social world—could alsocontribute to hostile intent attributions. Specifically,we argue that implicit theories of personality,which involve a theory about the fixedness versusmalleability of personality traits, can promote hostile intent attributions when adolescents are confronted with peer provocations of ambiguousintent.Implicit theories are core beliefs about the malleability of people’s traits, and they frame people’sinterpretations of events in their social worlds. As aresult, they play a role in shaping judgments andreactions to other people’s behaviors (Chiu et al.,1997; Erdley & Dweck, 1993; Yeager et al., 2011).For instance, adults with more of an entity theoryoften interpret minor behaviors as having a stronger positive or negative valence (Hong, Chiu,Dweck, & Sacks, 1997) and as more indicative ofunderlying moral character (Chiu et al., 1997), compared to adults with more of an incremental theory.Past research is consistent in showing that an entitytheory about personality creates a psychologicalworld in which people’s global character can bejudged as good or bad from even thin slices ofbehavior.Building on this research, we propose thatalthough an entity theory of personality itself doesnot necessarily arise from hostile influences, it cannevertheless create a psychological preparednesstoward hostile judgments of others. Consistent withthis prediction, Erdley and Dweck (1993, Study 2)found that fourth- and fifth-grade students whoheld more of an entity theory viewed a peer’s antisocial behavior as arising from the peer’s underlying, stable, and deficient traits. They thendisplayed less empathy for the peer and prescribedmore punishment for him or her. Moreover, whenlater confronted with positive behaviors performedby the peer—thus providing participants with anopportunity to view the peer’s previous antisocialbehavior as due to circumstances rather than traits—those with more of an entity theory maintained theirglobal negative trait judgments of the antisocial peer.More recently, Yeager et al. (2011) examined theeffect of implicit theories of personality on high

Implicit Theories and Hostile Intent Attributionsschool students’ responses to personal experiencesof peer conflict. They showed that those with moreof an entity theory were more likely to attributeinstances of victimization directed at them to apeer’s personal qualities. This characterologicalattribution mediated an effect on a greater desire to“get back at,” “hurt,” or “punish” the transgressingpeer. Next, Yeager et al. (2011) used a brief experiment to change adolescents’ implicit theoriestoward more of an incremental view, and showedthat the incremental theory reduced the belief that ahypothetical peer who bullied them had negativepersonal qualities. This, in turn, reduced the desirefor revenge.Although instructive, this past research did notinvestigate the role of implicit theories in shapingthe interpretation of negative events in which theintent of an anonymous peer was ambiguous—which is the critical test of an attributional bias (cf.Dodge, 1980). The Yeager et al. (2011) scenariosinvolved peers who explicitly and unambiguouslywere bullying the participants repeatedly on purpose. Thus, it is currently unknown whether implicit theories of personality would predict the hostileattributional bias. Moreover, both the Erdley andDweck (1993) and the Yeager et al. (2011) researchrelied on hypothetical scenarios and did not measure attributions following actual experiences ofpeer conflict. Therefore, the present studies extendedpast research by testing whether implicit theoriesmight predict attributions of hostile intent followingambiguous provocations from unknown peers inhypothetical scenarios and in controlled behavioralexperiences of peer exclusion.The Origins of Implicit TheoriesAlthough the developmental origins of an entitytheory are not fully known, some past research supports the notion that entity theories can result fromnonhostile or even positive influences, and yetnonetheless predict negative attributions and reactions. In analogous past experiments on implicittheories of intelligence, positive, well-intentionedpraise for being “smart” could induce an entitytheory of intelligence (Mueller & Dweck, 1998).Moreover, Dweck et al. (1995) reported that a fixedor entity theory about intelligence is not simply theresidue of poor academic performance. Many highachieving students hold an entity theory about theiracademic ability, yet those with more of an entitytheory of academic ability, regardless of their actuallevels of ability, tend to respond more negatively inthe face of academic challenges, for example, by3blaming their failures on themselves, lying aboutlow grades, considering cheating, or giving up(Blackwell, Trzesniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Mueller& Dweck, 1998; for an overview, see Dweck, 2006).More directly relevant to the present research, Chiuet al. (1997, Study 5) changed participants’ implicittheories of personality with a brief scientific articlesummarizing longitudinal studies that eithershowed that people’s traits become set, like plaster(in an entity theory condition) or that they can bemolded, like clay, throughout life (in an incrementaltheory condition). This brief article led participantsto adopt entity versus incremental frameworkswhen judging others. Hence, past research is suggestive that implicit theories can arise even frompositive or nonhostile influences (such as scientificinformation) and are not only the product of hostileexperiences. Building on this past research, we propose that adolescents who have learned more ofan entity theory of personality, regardless of thefrequency of past negative events or their overallnegative schemas about people, will respond toambiguous peer conflicts with a different pattern ofattributions and behavioral desires than those whohave learned more of an incremental theory.The Present ResearchStudy 1 reports 11 original correlations that werecollected from eight independent samples of adolescents. Five of these samples included one test of therelation between implicit theories and the hostileattributional bias, and three included two testsemploying measures collected at different times.These 11 tests of our hypothesis employed variousquestions, stimuli (e.g., scenarios and behavioralprovocations), and lengths of time between measurements, and they were aggregated into a singleeffect size estimate using meta-analytic methods, tofacilitate more general conclusions. In addition, totest for whether implicit theories would predictattributions among adolescents from relatively hostile as well as relatively nonhostile contexts, Study1 included samples from both lower violence andhigher violence neighborhoods. Last, Study 1 metaanalytically summarized tests of whether attributions of hostile intent mediated the effect of implicittheories on aggressive desires.Study 2 was an experiment designed to test thecausal effect of implicit theories on hostile attributional biases. In it we changed implicit theoriestoward more of a malleable or incremental view ofpersonality and then measured short-term changesin attributions of hostile intent. As in Study 1, this

4Yeager, Miu, Powers, and Dweckexperiment was conducted in two contexts withhighly different levels of neighborhood violence, todemonstrate the generality of this process. Finally,Study 3 was a longitudinal experiment that administered a brief (two class session) incremental theoryintervention and measured attributions of hostileintent and vengeful desires 8 months later.Study 1low socioeconomic status, and came from neighborhoods with violence rates above the national average, according to federal statistics (United StatesDepartment of Justice, Federal Bureau of Investigation, 2011). In the remaining two samples themajority of students came from middle- or highincome families and lived in neighborhoods withviolence rates below the national average. Responserates in all studies were over 75%. Parental consentand student assent were obtained for all ts were students in Grades 8, 9, and 10from eight independent samples coming from sixdifferent schools in the United States (samples collected from the same schools included different students surveyed in different years; N 1,128 uniqueparticipants provided a total of N 1,659 observations). See Table 1 for a summary of sample characteristics. Some of these studies were conductedexpressly to test the present hypotheses, and otherswere designed to test additional hypotheses thatwill be included in future research articles. However, critically, all the samples included here wereadministered measures of implicit theories andattributions of hostile intent, and none of the relations between these variables have been reported inanother research article.In the aggregate, 49% of participants werefemale. Twelve percent self-identified as Black orAfrican American, 17% as White, 26% as Asian,42% as Hispanic or Latino, and the rest as anotherrace or ethnicity. Schools were located in New YorkCity or the San Francisco Bay Area. In six of theeight samples, the majority of students at the schoolreceived free or reduced-price lunch, an indicator ofIn all studies, students completed activities andquestions as a class on computers in a school computer lab during school hours. The session wasadministered by a trained research assistant wholed students to work quietly and privately for theroughly 30 min required. Four of the studiesincluded an ambiguous provocation that studentsbelieved was a real interaction with another student(described below). In each case, students were thoroughly debriefed after the experience. At the end ofall of the studies, students were thanked for theirparticipation and were told how their responseswould be used to advance science and help futurestudents like themselves.MeasuresEntity TheoryParticipants in all studies answered five questions measuring an entity theory of personalitytraits relevant to peer conflicts in high school. Thesequestions were developed and used in past research(Yeager et al., 2011). Participants were asked toTable 1Samples Summarized Meta-Analytically in Study 1Sample12345678N391281792043630521126Grade n% female% Black% White% Asian% Hispanic or LatinoNew York, NYBay Area, CABay Area, CANew York, NYBay Area, CABay Area, CABay Area, CABay Area, 46610283858356158080Note. Participants could indicate as many races or ethnicities as they wished, or none at all, and therefore row percentages need notadd up to 100%. Samples 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, and 8 were collected from schools located in neighborhoods with higher levels of poverty andviolence, whereas samples 5 and 6 were located in upper-middle-class neighborhoods with lower levels of poverty and violence.

Implicit Theories and Hostile Intent Attributionsagree or disagree with statements such as “Bulliesand victims are types of people that really can’t bechanged” (1 strongly disagree, 6 strongly agree;below we report analyses showing that implicit theories are distinct from overall negative schemasabout people). These items were internally consistent across studies: as ranged from .66 to .83.Attributions of Hostile IntentIn each of the tests of our hypothesis, one of twodifferent ambiguous provocations was used toevoke attributions of hostile intent. One was ahypothetical scenario and one was a behavioralexperience of social exclusion. Following the designemployed in past demonstrations of an attributionalbias (e.g., Dodge, 1980), in each of these provocations the identity of the peer was unknown to theparticipant. Indeed, a crucial precondition for a testof an attributional bias is to examine person perceptions in the absence of any previous knowledgeabout the character of the provoking peer.Hypothetical scenario. Seven tests of our hypothesis employed an ambiguous scenario. This was aparadigmatic scenario used to illustrate the hostileattributional bias and was based on an exampledescribed by Dodge (2006, p. 791):Imagine that you were walking in a crowdedhallway at school and everybody was rushing toget to the next class so they wouldn’t be late.While you were looking the other way, you andanother student bumped into each other (prettyhard), so it hurt your shoulder and you droppedthe books that you were carrying. The other student paused briefly, looked at you quickly, andthen turned away and hurried to class.A pilot experiment showed that this stimuluswas, in fact, ambiguous with regard to hostileintent (see online Supporting Information).Cyberball exclusion. In four tests of our hypothesis,students experienced a standardized (but short-livedand harmless) instance of social exclusion duringan online game of catch (“Cyberball”) with two“peers” purportedly from their secondary schoolbut lo

entity theory would predict greater attributions of hostile intent following both imagined and experi-enced provocations. Study 1 also explored whether these increased hostile attributions might statisti-cally mediate the effect of an entity theory on the desire to respond aggressively. Study 2 addressed the causal role of implicit theories.

Related Documents:

INSIDE THE BLACK BOX: IMPLICIT LEADERSHIP THEORIES AND LEADERSHIP PROTOTYPES Both research in personality theory (Shweder, 197.5, 1982) and a book on implicit psy- chology (Wegner & Vallacher. 1977) argue that individuals develop implicit theories (implicit in the sense of not being spelled out) to give meaning to events. .

describe earlier published methods of creating interpolating implicit surfaces. 2.1 Implicit Surfaces An implicit surface is defined by an implicit function, a continuous scalar-valued function over the domainR3. The implicit surface of such a function is the locus of points at which the function takes on the value zero.

to other personality disorders such as antisocial personality disorder. Ogloff (2005) distinguishes psychopathy from antisocial personality disorder due to the emphasis on affective and personality rather than mostly behavioral elements of antisocial personality disorder. Besides antisocial personality disorder, there are other DSM-IV personality

Prescribed by ANSI Std Z39-18 . IMPLICIT PERSONALITY AND LEADERSHIP IN STRESSFUL AND DANGEROUS SITUATIONS: A FIRST STEP . Graduation Rate as Predicted by Implicit Aggression & Achievement B4 Figure 4: Attrition Over Time by Leader Personality (N 212) . (1967), who showed that an indirec

Understanding The Supporter Personality Chapter 5: Understanding The Promoting/Supporter Personality Chapter 6: Understanding The Promoter/Controller Personality Chapter 7: Understanding The Controller/Analyzer Personality Chapter 8 : Understanding The Analyzer/Supporter Personality Chapter 9: Understanding The Centric Personality Wrapping Up

the light of sound theoretical systems of personality. 3. To acquaint the students with the applications of personality theories in different walks of life. Unit Contents No. of Lectures Unit-I INTRODUCTION TO PERSONALITY 1.1.Definitions and nature of personality 1.2.Characteristics of good personality theory and Evaluation of personality theory

Implicit Theories and Self-Regulation 1 Mindsets Matter: A Meta-Analytic Review of Implicit Theories and Self-Regulation Abstract This review builds on self-control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1998) to develop a theoretical

RECENT and CLASSIC IMPLICIT BIAS LITERATURE REPORTS ON IMPLICIT BIAS 1. The Science of Equality, Volume 1: Addressing Implicit Bias, Racial Anxiety, and Stereotype Threat in Education and Health Care [Godsil, Tropp, Goff & Powell, 2014] . State of The Science ‐‐ Implicit Bias Review 2014 .File Size: 338KB