Intervention In Reading Processes In Pupils With Specifi C .

2y ago
7 Views
3 Downloads
333.59 KB
7 Pages
Last View : 1m ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Olive Grimm
Transcription

Víctor M. Acosta Rodríguez, Gustavo M. Ramírez Santana, Nayarit del Valle Hernández and Laura de Castro BermúdezPsicothema 2016, Vol. 28, No. 1, 40-46doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.144ISSN 0214 - 9915 CODEN PSOTEGCopyright 2016 Psicothemawww.psicothema.comIntervention in reading processes in pupils with Specific LanguageImpairment (SLI)Víctor M. Acosta Rodríguez, Gustavo M. Ramírez Santana, Nayarit del Valle Hernándezand Laura de Castro BermúdezUniversidad de La LagunaAbstractBackground: Children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)encounter significant difficulties in learning to read. The aim of thisresearch was to determine the efficacy of an intervention program onreading processes (letter identification, lexical processes, syntacticprocesses and semantic processes) in children with SLI. Method: Thesample consisted of a total of 34 pupils diagnosed with SLI and 34children with typical language development. For the selection of thesample, the CELF-3 test, the Peabody test, the Hearing Associationand Visual Association subtests of the ITPA and the K-BIT Intelligencetest were used. The intervention program consisted of 144 sessions of40 minutes each, in which oral language activities were combined withother activities related to the automation of basic reading processes andreading sentences and texts. Results: Significant gains were also made inthe group of children with SLI versus controls in lexical, syntactic, andsemantic reading processes. Conclusions: A combined program of bothoral language and reading skills improves reading achievement in pupilswith SLI.Keywords: reading, assessment, intervention programs, specific languageimpairment.During the early stages of education, reading is a fundamentalobjective for all pupils. However, there are risk groups who sufferserious difficulties for effective learning. These include childrenwith Specific Language Impairment (SLI), who are characterizedby having a considerable delay in their language development,despite the absence of neurological, sensory or non-verbalintelligence deficits (Leonard, 2014). Many studies have foundreading problems in these pupils (Catts, Fey, Tomblin, & Zhang,2002). In fact, oral language problems have a negative impacton reading, especially when phonological disturbances appeartogether with related lexical-semantic, morphosyntactic, andnarrative aspects (Coloma et al., 2012).Received: June 1, 2015 Accepted: November 24, 2015Corresponding author: Víctor M. Acosta RodríguezFacultad de Ciencias de la SaludUniversidad de La Laguna38350 Santa Cruz de Tenerife (Spain)e-mail: vacosta@ull.es40ResumenIntervención en procesos lectores en alumnado con Trastorno Específicodel Lenguaje (TEL). Antecedentes: los niños con Trastorno Específicodel Lenguaje (TEL) tienen importantes dificultades para el aprendizaje dela lectura. El objetivo de esta investigación ha sido comprobar la eficaciade un programa de intervención sobre los procesos lectores (identificaciónde letras, procesos léxicos, procesos gramaticales y procesos semánticos)en niños con TEL. Método: la muestra estuvo compuesta por un total de34 alumnos diagnosticados con TEL y 34 niños con un desarrollo típicodel lenguaje. Para la selección de la muestra se utilizaron los tests CELF-3,Peabody, las subpruebas de Asociación Auditiva y de Asociación Visualdel ITPA y el Test de Inteligencia K-BIT. El programa de intervenciónconstó de 144 sesiones, de 40 minutos de duración cada una, en el quese combinaron actividades de lenguaje oral con otras destinadas a laautomatización de procesos lectores básicos y de lectura de frases y textos.Resultados: los niños con TEL obtuvieron ganancias significativas frentea los controles en los procesos lectores léxicos, sintácticos y semánticos.Conclusiones: un programa combinado de lenguaje oral y lectura mejorael rendimiento lector en alumnado con TEL.Palabras clave: lectura, evaluación, programas de intervención, trastornoespecífico del lenguaje.Research data confirm that reading problems in SLI are mixed(Bishop & Snowling, 2004), that is, the causes should be soughtin the interaction of decoding and comprehension skills. First,a close relationship has been established between reading andphonological processing skills. In this sense, it was found thatphonological representations constitute an area of weakness inchildren with SLI, although not to the degree of severity reachedin other disorders such as dyslexia (Claessen & Leitao, 2012).Second, other components have been linked to reading difficultiesin these children, and more specifically with respect to their lexicalsemantic, grammatical and narrative deficits, that cause severeproblems of reading comprehension (Ramus, Marshall, Rosen,& van der Lely, 2013). Further, longitudinal studies confirm thatdifferences between children with SLI and their peers increaseover time (Flax et al., 2003).A review of the research on reading intervention shows thatinterventions combining phonological training with readingimprove skills in poor readers. However, there is less empiricalevidence of the effectiveness of intervention in children with SLI

Intervention in reading processes in pupils with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)with respect to difficulties in grammatical and semantic processes.In this case, it is important to train receptive and expressivelanguage skills to improve reading comprehension. Bowyer-Craneet al. (2008) studied the effectiveness of two training programs forimproving reading skills in children with SLI. The first combinedphonological awareness activities with reading books, while thesecond focused on helping improve oral language skills, thatis, vocabulary, inferencing, expressive language and listeningskills. The first program led to improved letter identificationand phonological awareness, while the second optimized lexicaland grammatical skills. Currently, most intervention programscombine stimulation of phonological awareness and reading skillswith oral language training (Hulme & Snowling, 2011; Clarke,Truelove, Hulme, & Snowling, 2014).The above considerations were taken into account whendesigning the present study for Spanish-speaking children withSLI. Specifically, the main objective was to improve readingprocesses of children with SLI by an intervention program.MethodParticipantsThe sample consisted of 68 primary school children, dividedinto two groups. An experimental or SLI group (SG) was composedof 34 children diagnosed with specific language impairment (28males, mean age: 8.01, range: 5.68-11.53, SD: 1.55 years). To makeup the control group (CG), 34 children with typical languagedevelopment were chosen from among the classmates of thechildren with SLI in order to homogenize the sample as muchas possible by eliminating variables such as the school context,teacher, methodology or peer group. These pupils had no languageproblems and followed schooling within the usual parameters (28males, mean age: 7.95 years, range: 5.72-11.43, SD 1.59).The normality of the age variable was verified by theKolmogorov-Smirnov test (z 1.022, df 68, p .247). To verifythat the groups were well matched in this variable, a hypothesistest was performed. A t-test showed no significant differencebetween the groups; also, the effect size was small, t(64) 1.25, p .879; Cohen’s d .04; Levene’s F(1, 66) .04, p .853.InstrumentsInstruments for the selection and evaluation of the sample.First, participants were subjected to certain exclusion criteriarelated to SLI present in the literature: namely, the pupils’ schoolhistories were examined to determine whether major problemsexisted, especially with respect to their hearing and orofacial motorskills. Then, the three tests set forth below were administered. Theresults of this process are set out in Table 1.CELF-3. Starting with the test most used internationally for thestudy of this disorder, the CELF-3 (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003).This is a language assessment test with scales for Spanish speakersin the United States, with Cronbach’s alpha between .74 and .91. Itevaluates the processes of language comprehension and expression ingeneral, by means of tasks involving the structuring and formulationof sentences, concepts and directions, structure and kinds of words,and remembering prayers. The results in this test, expressed instandard deviations below the mean, were crucial. However, giventhat some researchers point to the need to use two or more measuresof language, the Peabody test (Dunn, Padilla, Lugo, & Dunn,1986) was also used, as well as two subtests of the Illinois Test ofPsycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA; Kirk, McCarthy, & Kirk, 2005).PEABODY. The Peabody test, focused on vocabulary, can beadministered between 2.6 and 16 years of age, with a reliabilityof α .93. The child must choose from among four imagesthe one corresponding to the word given by the evaluator, andthe vocabulary used consists of names of objects, situations,professions and animals, actions and attributes. The SLI groupresults, expressed as standard deviations below the mean, werealso very low for this test.ITPA. Finally, we administered the Visual and HearingAssociation subtests of the ITPA (Cronbach’s alpha between .75 and.91) to check the degree of knowledge of conceptual relationships(semantic psycholinguistic processes); results showed a meanpsycholinguistic age far below the chronological age.Kaufman’s Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT; Kaufman &Kaufman, 2000). Furthermore, it was found that the children withSLI had a non-verbal IQ equal to or higher than the score of 85; forthis, the K-BIT Intelligence Test was used. This test was chosenbecause it uses non-verbal forms (α .98).Instruments for evaluating reading processes. Readingprocesses were studied using the PROLEC-R (Cuetos, Rodríguez,Ruano, & Arribas, 2009). This consists of two tests for each ofthe processes involved in reading, except semantic processes,which consists of three tests. The first two tests – the names orsounds of letters test and the same-different test – are intended toexamine the processes of letter identification; the next two tests,which entail reading words and nonwords, are used to assesslexical or visual word recognition processes; syntactic processesare evaluated by means of tests of grammatical structures andpunctuation marks; and finally, semantic processes are studied bytesting sentence comprehension, text comprehension and listening(oral comprehension). Reliability is established by means of aCronbach’s alpha coefficient between .48 and .74.ProcedureThe study used a quasi-experimental design of repeatedmeasures with a pretest-posttest control group (cross design withnon-equivalent control group). The intervention program was theindependent variable and reading processes – that is, identificationof letters and lexical, syntactic, and semantic processes –constituted the dependent variables.Table 1Test results for diagnostic evaluationSGCGTestsMSDMSDCELF Expressive. SD-1.4.91.5.9CELF Receptive. SD-1.2.61.1.7PEABODY. SD-1.51.2.3.9ITPA. Hearing Assoc. PA3.92.37.72.0ITPA. Visual Assoc. PA5.42.36.61.7K-BIT. Non-verbal IQ102.29.0114.912.3Note: SG: Pupils with SLI; CG: Control group; PA: Psycholinguistic age; SD: Standarddeviation; IQ: Intelligence quotient.41

Víctor M. Acosta Rodríguez, Gustavo M. Ramírez Santana, Nayarit del Valle Hernández and Laura de Castro BermúdezParticipants selection. An initial screening was performed inall schools in the Island of Tenerife, in collaboration with teamsof educational psychologists, who were asked to identify all pupilswith potential features of SLI, that is, those with problems with oneor more components of language expression and/or comprehension,especially morphosyntax and semantics, or who had spentseveral years with unresolved language difficulties. A total of65 pupils were identified, who were then given a comprehensiveevaluation protocol to confirm the diagnosis, consisting of variousstandardized tests. Thirty-one pupils were found to present only asimple language delay, that is, a slight time lag with phonologicalproblems but no lexical-semantic or morphosyntactic disturbances.The final sample consisted of pupils from different socioeconomicbackgrounds, attending both public and private schools, and fromboth rural and metropolitan areas. Finally, parents/guardians wereasked to give their consent to the child’s participation in the studyby signing the corresponding informed consent.The intervention program. The chronological age controlgroup received no intervention of any sort; the SLI group receivedan intervention program during the 2012-13 and 2013-14 schoolyears, with an overall duration of 18 months. A total of 144sessions lasting 40 minutes each were conducted with a twice-Table 2Components, activities, materials, and procedures of the intervention ative Reading the story The three hungry micewithout and with icons Retelling the story Inventing a story with help Generating own stories, without adultassistance Comic strips showing the story Icons representing the basic categories ofthe narrative structure Cards and pictograms Sticks for generating stories Offering of models; using questionsat different levels of complexity, multipleopportunities to respond Recast, expansions, extensions and verticalstructure Graphic organizersMorphosyntax Completing sentences Crossing out the incorrect words insentences Sorting sentences Ordering and verbalizing patternedsequences of actions with drawing Placing phrases in their respective speechbubbles Sorting phrases aided by a card Using support graphics to associate a phrasewith a drawing Comics para Hablar (Monfort & Juárez,1988) Comprender el lenguaje haciendo ejercicios(Aguado, Cruz, & Domezáin, 2003) Logo-kit 1 (Monfort, Juárez, & Monfort,2008) The recast as an immediate response to whatis produced by the child, but modifying one ofthe elements of the sentence or mode Modeling Descriptions and reviews of objects Imitations Building a story through drawingsIdentifying letters and lexicalprocesses Phonological awareness Naming: colors, drawing and numbers Reading words and nonwords Programa ALE (González & Cuetos, 2008) The processes are contextualized throughcharacters that guide all activitiesReading Level 1. Explanation of the unit theme (e.g.,transportation)-Vocabulary work- Sentence construction- Guessing concepts from definitions- Phonological awareness- Phoneme-grapheme associationLevel 2. Again, explanation of the unit theme(e.g., transportation)-Vocabulary: medium frequency words (boat,submarine, etc.) and action phrases- Conversation- Introduction of verbs (to fly, to travel, etc.)- Phonological awareness- Phoneme-grapheme association- Games with letters- Fast reading of letters- Reading syllables and wordsLevel 3. Again, explanation of the unit theme- Vocabulary: low frequency words (ballooning,paragliding, etc.)- Expand the semantic field with related terms- Phonological awareness- Reading: different fonts, reading paragraphs,phoneme-grapheme and orthographicrepresentations of word conversion (readingfluency)- Reading games with letters and words- Reading sentences and text Leer en un clic (García de Castro & Cuetos,2012). Each level has its own mascot to motivatethe child.Level 1: Dompi the elephant; Level 2: Petra thebear; Level 3: Ara and Bruna The characters will guide the variousactivities42

Intervention in reading processes in pupils with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)weekly frequency; the aim of these sessions was to promote pupils’reading and oral language skills. The work was always done inthe morning in the speech therapy classroom of each participatingschool, and was administered by a total of 18 speech/languagetherapists.A series of joint seminars and workshops (involving theresearch group and speech therapists) were held to establish theobjectives, content, and procedures for the intervention and selectthe materials required. Once the speech therapists had been fullyfamiliarized with the program, they began its implementation, withregular monitoring conducted by means of direct observation viavideo recordings. Subsequently, these recordings were analyzed injoint seminars in order to resolve the difficulties encountered andassess the achievements made, thereby ensuring the reliability ofthe intervention. In this regard, it was found that the interventionwas indeed conducted according to plan throughout the courseof the program, with the different speech therapists involvedfollowing the same guidelines.The aim of the intervention was to stimulate oral languageskills and basic reading processes. For the first objective, a seriesof oral storytelling activities were designed using fictional storiesin order to improve flow, production, and comprehension of orallanguage. Along with this, other activities were also carried outaimed at optimizing the participants’ knowledge, comprehension,and production of different syntactic structures.For the second objective, the ALE program (González &Cuetos, 2008) was used in order to automate the processes ofpreparation for reading (phonological awareness and namingspeed) and recognizing letters, syllables, words and nonwords(lexical processes). The rest of the work involved implementingthe method Leer en un clic (García de Castro & Cuetos, 2012).This method offers a number of phonological awareness activities,where each grapheme is formed from a drawing that has theshape of the grapheme and that starts with that sound, so that therelationship is no longer arbitrary. It also contains a large numberof activities to contribute to increasing pupils’ vocabulary. Thework plan is organized into three levels. In our case, we started atLevel 1, as the levels do not correspond to chronological age butto reading levels, determined using the PROLEC-R (Cuetos et al.,2009), as discussed above. The three levels deal with the samesemantic units and letters, the main difference being the numberof words and pictures used, as well as the use of written texts thatincrease in complexity as one moves up the levels. A guide fortherapists (Suárez & Cuetos, 2013: 5) gives a detailed explanationof the procedures to be followed:Key aspects of learning to read are covered: vocabulary andfluency in lexical access, phonological awareness, graphemephoneme rules, and orthographic representations of words. Finally,depending on the level, pupils will then begin with reading letters,syllables, words, sentences or texts. A different letter drawing iscreated for each semantic field; for example, for the transport unit, adrawing of an airplane (“avión”) is transformed into the letter “a”.Table 2 shows the different components of the interventionprogram.Data analysisTo evaluate the effects of the intervention program on thedependent variables (PROLEC factors), that is, Identificationof letters, Lexical processes, Syntactic processes, and Semanticprocesses, a descriptive analysis and multivariate analysis ofcovariance (MANCOVA) were run for each. Further, univariateanalyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were performed for eachcomponent (PROLEC major indices) of these dependent variables:Identification of letters (letter names, same-different), Lexicalprocesses (reading words, reading nonwords), Syntactic processes(grammatical structures, punctuation marks), and Semanticprocesses (sentence comprehension, text comprehension,oral comprehension). Also, age was used as a covariate in allanalyses.First, the MANCOVA and ANCOVAs were performed withpretest scores. Second, the MANCOVA and ANCOVA wereperformed with posttest scores, adding the pretest score as asecond covariate. Finally, MANCOVA and ANCOVA wereperformed with pretest-posttest differences. For each variable, inthe pretest, posttest, and pretest-posttest differences, effect sizewas also calculated. Statistical analyses were performed usingSPSS for MAC, v. 21.0.ResultsAnalysis PretestFirst, the descriptive analysis was run of the pretest scores ofboth groups (see Table 3), with a MANCOVA for each dependentvariable (see Table 4) and an ANCOVA with each component ofthe dependent variables (see Table 5).The pretest MANCOVA for Identification of letters showedsignificant differences and a large effect size. Results of the pretestANCOVAs indicated that before the intervention, the differencesbetween the groups were statistically significant and effect sizeswere large in both components of this dependent variable.The pretest MANCOVA for Lexical processes showedsignificant differences and a large effect size. Results of thepretest ANCOVAs indicated significant differences in the twocomponents, and the effect sizes were large.The pretest MANCOVA for Syntactic processes showedsignificant differences and a large effect size. Results of the pretestANCOVAs showed statistically significant differences betweengroups and the effect sizes were large in both components.The pretest MANCOVA for Semantic processes showedsignificant differences and a large effect size. Results of the pretestANCOVAs showed statistically significant differences betweenthe groups in all three components and all effect sizes were large.Analysis PosttestSubsequently, the analyses with posttest scores of the dependentvariables were made, adding the pretest score as a second covariate.The results are presented in Tables 3, 4 and 5.The posttest MANCOVA for Identification of letters showedno significant difference and a moderate effect size. Results ofthe posttest ANCOVAs indicated that differences between groupswere not obtained for any component of this dependent variable,and both effect sizes were moderate.The posttest MANCOVA for Lexical processes showed nosignificant difference and a moderate effect size. None of theposttest ANCOVAs for the components of this variable indicatedsignificant differences, and both effect sizes were small ormoderate.43

Víctor M. Acosta Rodríguez, Gustavo M. Ramírez Santana, Nayarit del Valle Hernández and Laura de Castro BermúdezTable 3Descriptives of principal indexes of PROLEC-R in the pretest, posttest and pretest-posttest differences for each groupPretestComponents of the er names41.640.6117.6Same-different12.38.821.8Reading words32.334.1Reading nonwords22.021.7Grammatical structures6.9Puntuation .7Sentence Text 0Oral tes: SG: Pupil group with SLI (n 34); CG: Control group (n 34).Table 4Results of the multivariate analysis of covariances for each dependent variablebetween experimental and control groups.PrePostPost-PreDependent variablesFpηp2Fpηp2Fpηp2Identification of letters28.6.001.481.7.18.07.5.610.02Lexical processes13.0.001.302.4.11.094.9.011.16Syntactic processes28.7.001.493.8.03.133.4.041.12Semantic processes46.3.001.701.5.22.095.5.002.24Table 5Results of the univariate analysis of covariances each component of thedependent variables between experimental and control groups.Components of thedependent cation of lettersLetter 7.8.001.311.0.322.02.4.536.01Reading words25.1.001.29.1.709.00.8.371.02Reading nonwords24.7.001.291.7.197.038.7.005.14Lexical processesSyntactic processesGrammatical on .161.047.9.007.13Semantic processesSentencecomprehensionText comprehension81.5.001.57.7.392.0112.0.001.18Oral comprehension84.6.001.588.2.006.14.0.993.00The posttest MANCOVA for Semantic processes showed nosignificant difference and a moderate effect size. Statisticallysignificant differences were only found between groups in thelistening component (oral comprehension) with a large effect size.The other two components showed a small or moderate effectsize.Analysis Posttest-PretestFinally, the posttest-pretest differences were analyzed for eachdependent variable, with the results presented in Tables 3, 4 and 5.The post-pre MANCOVA for Identification of letters showedno significant differences and a small effect size. The resultsof the post-pre ANCOVAs indicated that no component of thisdependent variable showed differences between groups, and botheffect sizes were small.The post-pre MANCOVA for Lexical processes showedsignificant differences and moderate-large effect size. Onlythe post-pre ANCOVAs for the nonword reading componentindicated significant differences and moderate effect size. Theother component showed a small effect size.The post-pre MANCOVA for Syntactic processes showedsignificant differences and a moderate effect size. The results ofthe post-pre ANCOVAs showed statistically significant differencesbetween the groups only in the grammatical structures componentand a moderate effect size. The other component showed a smalleffect size.The post-pre MANCOVA for Semantic processes showedsignificant differences and a large effect size. Statisticallysignificant differences between groups were found in the sentencecomprehension and text comprehension components, and botheffect sizes were moderate-large. The other component showed asmall effect size.DiscussionThe posttest MANCOVA for Syntactic processes showedsignificant differences and a moderate-large effect size. Results ofthe posttest ANCOVAs showed statistically significant differencesbetween groups in both components, and both effect sizes weremoderate-large or large.44According to the two-dimensional model proposed by Bishopand Snowling (2004), in which a close relationship is establishedbetween reading and language disorders, phonological deficitsaffect decoding skills, while deficits in other components of

Intervention in reading processes in pupils with Specific Language Impairment (SLI)language constitute risk factors for reading comprehensionproblems. In this sense, we should reflect on the first hypothesisof the present research. The results of the initial evaluation areworse in the SLI group than in the control group, with significantdifferences in all reading processes evaluated by the PROLEC-R(Cuetos et al., 2009). This corroborates the presence of mixedreading problems in the experimental group, with deficits thataffect both decoding and comprehension, in line with data fromother studies with Spanish-speaking children (Coloma et al.,2012).The second hypothesis was linked to the effectiveness of theintervention program on the reading processes of children withSLI. The results obtained in our study are similar to those of authorslike Hulme and Snowling (2011), whose proposals combined orallanguage activities and reading texts. More specifically, it shouldbe noted that significant gains were observed in precisely thoseprocesses and tasks that are most vulnerable in these pupils. Thus,for example, they clearly improved their nonword reading. Muchresearch has been done on the limitations that SLI places on theability to store information in the phonological working memory,a fact that hinders phonological representations and consequentlyweakens acquisition of vocabulary, morphosyntax and readingfluency (Leonard, 2014). The activities aimed at optimizinggrapheme-phoneme association, naming speed and reading wordsand nonwords in both the ALE program (González & Cuetos,2008) and Leer en un clic (García de Castro & Cuetos, 2012) led toconsiderable improvements in reading achieved by the sublexicalmeans.Significant differences were also obtained in the syntacticprocesses, and more specifically in grammatical structures. Notsurprisingly, the core of linguistic problems in SLI coincideswith morphosyntactic problems (Washington, 2013); therefore,improving knowledge of the syntactic roles of the words that makeup sentences and syntactic processing are important. In this sense,a decisive factor in the intervention program was the incorporationof activities linked to developing narrative and morphosyntax.Finally, children in the SLI group made considerablegains in semantic processes, specifically in sentence and textcomprehension, aspects that receive special attention in levels 2and 3 of Leer en un clic (García de Castro & Cuetos, 2012). Whileit is important to understand different types of sentences, thelinguistic profile of this disorder is such that improvement in textcomprehension is considered more fundamental, as it indicates anability to extract meaning from texts and integrate it into one’s ownknowledge. However, there was no major progress in listening (oralcomprehension). The explanation for this finding could be relatedto the usual limitations faced by children with SLI in temporalauditory speech processing, in the resources available in workingmemory, and in the management of inferences. In this sense, wewould suggest incorporating into the program more attention to theteaching of vocabulary and improving working memory (LópezEscribano, Elosúa Gómez-Veiga, & García-Madruga, 2013); thegeneration of inferences (Hulme & Snowling, 2011); and finally,the formulation of questions and greater use of figurative language(Clarke, Truelove, Hulme, & Snowling, 2014).AcknowledgementsThis work was developed through funding from the Ministryof Economy and Competitiveness of the Government of Spain,under Research Project “Executive functions and language inpupils with SLI. A model of assessment and intervention withpsycholinguistic and neuropsychological bases”. Reference no.EDU2011-27789.ReferencesAguado, G., Ripoll, J., & Domezáin, M. (2003). Comprender el lenguajeha

of language, the Peabody test (Dunn, Padilla, Lugo, & Dunn, 1986) was also used, as well as two subtests of the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA; Kirk, McCarthy, & Kirk, 2005). PEABODY. The Peabody test, focused on vocabulary, can be administered betwee

Related Documents:

All About the Alphabet Reading Alphabet Fun: A Reading Alphabet Fun: B Reading Alphabet Fun: C Reading Alphabet Fun: D Reading Alphabet Fun: E Reading Alphabet Fun: F Reading Alphabet Fun: G Reading Alphabet Fun: H Reading Alphabet Fun: I Reading Alphabet Fun: J Reading Alphabet Fun: K Reading Alphabet Fu

student (e.g., an intervention to address reading fluency was chosen for a student whose primary deficit was in reading fluency). If the intervention is group-based, all students enrolled in the Tier 2/3 intervention group have a shared intervention need that could reasonably be addressed through the group instruction provided.

Subsea Intervention Type 1 (Class A) - Light (riserless) Intervention Type 2 (Class B) - Medium Intervention Type 3 (Class C) -Heavy Intervention Well and manifold installation Maintenance -scale squeeze -chemical injection Increasing demand for mature fields Image Ref - Subsea Well Intervention Vessel and Systems

reading at or above grade level. The reading needs of the population of students in need of intervention are so significant that additional support above and beyond reading in language arts and other content areas is necessary. Students reading at or above grade level will also benefit from explicit reading instruction at a developmental level.

The Academic or Behavioral Intervention Plan 105 . Step 6: Implementing the Intervention Plan and Process 107 . and Intervention Team (SPRINT) Consultation 325 . Referral Audit Form . Appendix III: The Project ACHIEVE/SPRINT Team End-of-the-Year Get-Go/Student 327 . Results of the Curriculum and Intervention Resource Survey Completed by the .

Limitations of Brief Intervention vs. Full Intervention Agenda of the family/team gets addressed as primary agenda vs. agenda of Interventionist. Most families call for an intervention when they are in partial or full crisis mode and want immediate intervention. Interventionist joins with family in expediting the process which may lessen the desired outcomes for family

5 17 SIPPS Intervention Programs 18 SIPPS Intervention K-3 Publisher: Developmental Studies Center Website: www.devstu.org Focus Population: 1 and 2; Intervention 2 and 3 Level of Intervention: Supplemental and Intense Number of Levels: Beginning, Extension, Challenge Materials: Teacher’s Manual, Spelling-

deepwater well intervention system offers a means to safely connect in a 6-3/8 inch borehole, from a vessel or rig on the water's surface, to subsea wells up to 15,000 psi in water as deep as 10,000 feet (3,000 m). Trident-OVS 15K Light Well Intervention System Subsea Systems Trident-OVS Light Well Intervention System 3 BOOST PRODUCTION