Par White Paper Brief - Psychological Assessment Resources

1y ago
4 Views
1 Downloads
2.14 MB
24 Pages
Last View : 1m ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Camille Dion
Transcription

Enhanced Interpretation of theBehavior Rating Inventory ofExecutive Function–PreschoolVersion (BRIEF-P)Jennifer A. Greene, PhDSue Trujillo, MSPeter K. Isquith, PhDGerard A. Gioia, PhDKimberly Andrews Espy, PhD

Executive SummaryThere are several steps to describing the strengths and weaknesses of a child’s everyday executive functioning via BehaviorRating Inventory of Executive Function–Preschool Version(BRIEF-P) ratings: assessing validity of ratings, making normative comparisons, examining base rates of scale/index scoresin normative and clinical groups, interpreting profiles of scaleelevations within a protocol, and interpreting ratings betweenraters (e.g., parents and teachers). These interpretive steps aresupported by the use of normative information provided in theBRIEF-P Professional Manual as well as the additional baserates, T-score profiles, and interrater statistics provided inAppendix A, B, and C of this white paper. While assessingvalidity and interpreting T scores are common practice, theadditional steps offered here provide for enhanced, and morenuanced, interpretation of BRIEF-P ratings. In addition toincorporating evidence from other sources, following thesesteps offers a more comprehensive evaluation of the child’sself-regulatory functioning across school and home environments. Enhanced BRIEF-P interpretation is demonstrated viaan illustrative case example.2

IntroductionThe Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–PreschoolVersion (BRIEF-P; Gioia, Espy, & Isquith, 2003) is a rating scale forparents and teachers of preschool-aged children that assesses everydaybehaviors associated with executive functions in the home and preschool environments. It is designed for preschool children ages 2 years,0 months to 5 years, 11 months, including those with emergent learningdisabilities and attentional disorders; language disorders; traumatic braininjuries; autism spectrum disorders; and other developmental, neurological, psychiatric, and medical conditions.Quick LinksAppendix A: BRIEF-P Base Ratesof Clinically Elevated T ScoresAppendix B: BRIEF-P MeanT ScoresAppendix C: Interrater StatisticsThe BRIEF-P is part of the BRIEF family of products, which includesthe BRIEF Second Edition (BRIEF2; Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy,2015) and the BRIEF–Adult Version (BRIEF-A; Roth, Isquith, & Gioia,2005). The BRIEF2 Parent and Teacher forms were developed forparents and teachers of children ages 5 to 18 years, and the BRIEF2Self-Report Form is for adolescents ages 11 to 18 years. The BRIEF-ASelf-Report Form and Informant Form are used with adults ages 18years and older. Since the first BRIEF product was published in 2000,the family of instruments has been translated or adapted for use in morethan 60 languages on six continents. Additionally, more than 1,300studies published in peer-reviewed journals have included the BRIEFfamily of instruments, adding to a large international body of evidencefor reliable and valid interpretation with typically developing individualsand individuals with a broad spectrum of clinical conditions across thelife span. Since it was published in 2003, the BRIEF-P has beenincluded in more than 250 studies published internationally in peerreviewed journals, attesting to its validity for assessing the developmentof executive functions in very young children.The purpose of this white paper is to:a. Provide BRIEF-P users with new statistics to enhanceBRIEF-P interpretation, including interrater agreementmetrics and base rate tables for various clinical groupsand the standardization samples.b. Demonstrate enhanced BRIEF-P interpretation via anillustrative case example.An introduction to the BRIEF-P and executive function will first bepresented, followed by the steps for enhanced BRIEF-P interpretationand the case example. BRIEF-P statistics are provided in Appendix A, B,and C at the end of this white paper for use in your own interpretationof BRIEF-P scores.For professionals working with older children and adolescents, anin-depth guide to enhanced interpretation for the school-age BRIEF2Parent, Teacher, and Self-Report Forms is available in the BRIEF2Interpretive Guide (Isquith, Gioia, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2017), which canbe purchased on parinc.com.3Since it was publishedin 2003, the BRIEF-Phas been included inmore than 250 studiespublished internation ally in peer-reviewedjournals.

What is Executive Function?The executive functions are a collection of processes that are responsible forguiding, directing, and managing cognitive, emotional, and behavioral functions,particularly during novel problem solving. The term executive function represents anumbrella construct that includes a collection of interrelated functions responsible forpurposeful, goal-directed, problem-solving behavior.Specific subdomains that make up this collection of regulatory or managementfunctions include the ability to initiate behavior, inhibit competing actions or stimuli,select relevant task goals, plan and organize a means to solve complex problems,shift problem-solving strategies flexibly when necessary, and monitor and evaluatebehavior. The working memory capacity, whereby information is actively held“online” in the service of complex, multistep problem solving, is also described as akey aspect of executive function (Pennington, Bennetto, McAleer, & Roberts, 1996).Finally, the executive functions are not exclusive to cognitive control but also includeregulatory control of emotional response and behavioral action. Because executivefunction develops over time in typically developing children relative to the structuraland functional development of the brain, it is important to quantify what is atypicalexecutive functioning given a child’s age and also recognize that executive dysfunction can be an indication of other diagnoses.The termexecutive functionrepresents anumbrellaconstruct thatincludes acollection of inter related functionsresponsible forpurposeful, goaldirected, problemsolving behavior.The BRIEF-PThe BRIEF-P contains 63 items within five clinical scales that measure differentaspects of executive functioning: Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Working Memory,and Plan/Organize. Table 1 describes the clinical scales and two validity scales(Inconsistency and Negativity). The clinical scales form the three broader indexes ofInhibitory Self-Control (ISCI), Flexibility (FI), and Emergent Metacognition (EMI) andan overall composite score, the Global Executive Composite (GEC).Enhanced Interpretation of the BRIEF-PStrategies for interpreting the BRIEF-P scales are provided in the BRIEF-P Profes sional Manual. The following section describes an enhanced interpretation approachas outlined in Table 2. Table 2 provides the key steps for interpreting the BRIEF-Pand includes associated references and examples of statements that might beincluded in a report for each step. These steps are illustrated via a case exampleintroduced in the BRIEF-P Professional Manual and expanded on in the followingsections. Tables to aid in interpretation are found in the BRIEF-P Professional Manualas well in Appendix A, B, and C of this white paper.Case Example: AdamBackground InformationAdam is a 3-year, 8-month-old boy who presents with marked impulsivity,hyperactivity, and distractibility. His medical and developmental histories are benign,but he has a strong family history of attentional and behavioral disorders, and hisparents divorced when he was 1 year of age. Adam’s impulsivity has resulted in4

Table 1Description of the BRIEF-P ScalesScale/indexN of itemsClinical scale/indexInhibitDescription16 Controls impulses and behavior; appropriately stops and modulatesown behavior at the proper time or in the proper context10 Moves freely from one situation, activity, or aspect of a problem toanother as the situation demands; makes transitions; solves problemsflexibly10 Modulates emotional responses appropriately to situational demandor context17 Holds information in mind for the purpose of completing a task ormaking the appropriate response; stays with, or sticks to, an activity10 Anticipates future events or consequences; uses goals or instructionsto guide behavior in context; develops or implements appropriatesteps ahead of time to carry out an associated task or action26Composed of the Inhibit and Emotional Control scales20Composed of the Shift and Emotional Control scales27Composed of the Working Memory and Plan/Organize scales63 Composed of all clinical scales (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control,Working Memory, and Plan/Organize)ShiftEmotional ControlWorking MemoryPlan/OrganizeInhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI)Flexibility Index (FI)Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI)Global Executive Composite (GEC)Validity scalesInconsistency10 pairs Indicates the extent to which the respondent answers similar BRIEF-Pitems in an inconsistent manner10 Measures the extent to which the respondent answers selectedBRIEF-P items in an unusually negative mannerNegativityTable 2BRIEF-P InterpretationStepReferenceProcedureTables 2-3a andBRIEF-P ScoringSummary/ProfileFormReview Inconsistency and Negativityscales and other indications ofcompromised validity.Ratings on the BRIEF-P were valid.Appendixes AReview and report BRIEF-P T scoresaand B , BRIEF-Pand percentiles for scales, indexes,Score or Interpreand GEC.tive ReportParent ratings noted difficulties onthe Inhibit, Working Memory, andPlan/Organize scales but functioning was typical on the Shift andEmotional Control scales.1. Examine validity2a. Interpret scoresrelative to normativeexpectations2b. Examine base ratesAppendix A3. Interpret within-testAppendix Bscore profileaExample statementsCompare T scores to base-rate tablesof typically developing children andchildren with various acquired anddevelopmental disorders.Elevations of this magnitude on theInhibit and Working Memory scalesoccur in less than 10% of typicallydeveloping children his age.Review and report BRIEF-P T-scorepeaks and valleys; examine profilerelative to diagnostic groups.The profile pattern is like that seenin students diagnosed with ADHD.4. Interpret ratings betweenAppendix CinformantsExamine discrepancies between raters;Teacher and parent ratings revealedconsider interrater reliabilities, basea similar pattern of concerns withrates and significance levels ofinhibitory control, working memory,differences, and possible explanations. and planning and organizationbut also suggested problems withemotional control in the classroomsetting. Teacher and parent ratings were ingood agreement in general.Note. GEC Global Executive Composite.aAppears in Gioia, Espy, & Isquith (2003).5

several accidents that required doctors’ officevisits for stitches, but none with alteration ofconsciousness. He is quick to hit, kick, orthrow things when his needs are not met.Because of his behavior, he has been askedto leave two day care programs, and hismother now stays at home to care for him.Adam’s pediatrician referred him for anevaluation of his current difficulties anddevelopment of intervention recommendations. During his evaluation, he demonstrateda broad range of affect that was mercurial,ranging from excitement at finding a newobject in the office to anger when not allowedto leave the room on demand. As part of theevaluation, both parent (Adam’s mother) andteacher (Adam’s former day care teacher)BRIEF-P ratings were obtained; their scoresare presented in Figure 1.Steps for BRIEF-P InterpretationStep 1: Examine ValidityBefore interpreting BRIEF-P parent orteacher scores, the clinician should carefullyconsider the validity of the data provided. Theinherent nature of rating scales (i.e., relianceon a third party for ratings of a child’sbehav ior) potentially introduces bias to thescores. The BRIEF-P includes two scales(Inconsistency and Negativity) that provideinformation about validity.Adam’s parent and teacher Inconsistencyscores were in the Acceptable range, but theparent Negativity score was Elevated. Thisscore raised the possibility of overly negativeviews by his mother, who completed thescale, but it must also be viewed in thecontext of Adam’s fairly extreme behaviors.Adam’s mother rated him as often havingdifficulties on many items, with the exceptionof items on the Shift scale. Because of theconsistency among the ratings, his history ofexpulsion from day care programs, and hismarked impulsivity and activity level duringthe evaluation, the ratings likely reflectextreme behaviors rather than an overlynegative rater perspective. Indeed, Adam’sbehaviors were extreme, and the Negativityscale was designed to capture behaviors thatare rarely endorsed except in cases ofextreme behaviors or negative bias.It is important to note that how, or whether, we report informationabout validity of ratings should be approached with care. If validityscales are not elevated, the simple statement parent and teacher ratingson the BRIEF-P were valid will suffice. Noting that a score was not validmay not be necessary or helpful. For example, writing parent ratingswere overly negative or teacher ratings were inconsistent can havenegative consequences for the relationship between parent, teacher,and clinician. Thus, it is often preferable for clinicians to review thevalidity scales on the BRIEF-P and to follow up via interview when thescales are elevated or questionable rather than to state specifics in thereport. Information about validity is meant to assist the clinician ininterpreting scores, not necessarily to provide feedback to the familyor educational team.Step 2a: Interpret Scores Relative to Normative ExpectationsAdam’s mother’s ratings on the BRIEF-P Inhibit and Emotional Controlscales were clinically elevated. Likewise, the ISCI score, which is composed of these two scales (see Figure 1), was also clinically elevated.Adam’s teacher rated Adam as clinically elevated on the Inhibit scaleand ele vated but within normal limits on the Emotional Control scale.Scores for both Adam’s mother and his teacher on the Working Memoryand Plan/Organize scales were also clinically elevated, as was the EMIscore. Of interest, the score on the Shift scale was not elevated foreither rater, suggesting that Adam does not exhibit behavioral rigidityor cognitive inflexibility. Indeed, one of Adam’s difficulties is that hehas no routines and does not adhere to the same patterns of dailyfunctioning—behaviors that are opposite of those captured on the Shiftscale. Because the Emotional Control scale score was elevated and theShift scale score was not, the associated FI score was only moderatelyelevated for both the parent and teacher ratings. In sum, Adam’s parentand teacher BRIEF-P scores suggest marked inhibitory control deficits.Because Adam does not have adequate ability to inhibit, his behaviorsare impulsive and his emotions are volatile. Further, he is unable tosustain working memory, reflected in his inability to remain attentiveor focused for reasonable lengths of time. In Adam’s report, we mightwrite: Parent and teacher ratings of Adam’s everyday executive functioning indicated marked problems inhibiting impulses, sustaining workingmemory and attention, and planning and organizing problem solving.Parent ratings also indicated marked problems regulating emotions.Step 2b: Examine Base RatesBRIEF-P T scores and percentiles provide information about the levelof concern relative to typically developing peers. The base rate of agiven score brings an important context to the score by highlightinghow often similar scores occur in typically developing children versuschildren with clinical conditions. Base rates of clinically elevatedT scores ( 65) for the BRIEF-P parent and teacher standardizationsamples as well as clinical groups (ADHD, ASD, TBI, SLI, DS, andpreterm birth) are presented in Appendix A. Given Adam’s presentation,BRIEF-P T scores from Adam’s mother can be compared to those in theBRIEF-P parent standardization sample and to children with ADHD andASD (see Figure 1). In Adam’s report, we could write: Elevations of thismagnitude (T 65) on the Inhibit, Working Memory, and Plan/Organize6

783657579Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI)Flexibility Index (FI)Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI)Global Executive Composite (GEC)Clinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedWithin normal limitsClinically elevatedQualitative 753188ASD808260798678635484T scoreClinically elevatedClinically elevatedWithin normal limitsClinically elevatedClinically elevatedClinically elevatedWithin normal limitsWithin normal limitsClinically elevatedQualitative labelBRIEF-P TeacherFigure 1. Results from the BRIEF-P Parent and Teacher Form ratings for Adam. ADHD attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASD autism spectrumdisorder.727572Emotional ControlPlan/Organize55ShiftWorking Memory86T scoreInhibitScale/index/compositeBase ratesBRIEF-PstandardizationBRIEF-P Parent

scales are seen in less than 10% oftypically developing children but arecommonly seen in children diagnosedwith attention disorders. The elevationon the Emotional Control scale isalso seen in less than 10% of typi cally developing children but only inapproximately 50% of childrenwith ADHD.Step 3: Interpret Within-TestScore ProfileScores on the BRIEF-P scalesprovide information about the level ofconcern compared with children inthe standardization sample. It isoften useful to interpret scoresrelative to other scales within aprofile, or to examine the peaks andvalleys within a single protocol and tocompare this profile to profiles inknown clinical groups such as inchildren with ADHD or ASD. Clinicalexperience suggests that it is unusualto find a flat profile across BRIEF-Pscales (i.e., all scales with similarT-score levels) for an individualreferred for evaluation. Rather, mostratings of children have peaks andvalleys that reflect areas of relativelygreater concern and areas of moretypical function.Figure 2 plots Adam’s BRIEF-Pparent T scores along with meanT scores from the BRIEF-P parentstandardization sample and childrendiagnosed with ADHD and ASD.Most ratings of childrenhave peaks and valleysthat reflect areas ofrelatively greaterconcern and areas ofmore typical function.Step 4: Interpret Ratings BetweenInformantsGathering multiple perspectives inthe assessment of a child’s functioning provides a more comprehensiveset of data with which to understandhis or her needs, with similarities anddifferences between raters oftenproviding clinically useful information.In the most clear-cut cases, eachinformant will have a generally similarperspective with overall agreementacross scales and indexes. A morechallenging case occurs when thereis disagreement. There may beseveral reasons for differencesbetween ratings, and these reasonsmay lead to different interventions.Appendix B presents the meanT scores for these and various otherclinical groups. Visual inspectionshows that Adam’s scores are moresimilar to those of children withADHD than to children with ASDor to typically developing children.Comparing Adam’s scores to bothclinical profiles reveals that his Inhibit9085scale score is highly elevated, whichis similar to children with eitherADHD or ASD. However, his low Shiftscale score is more like the profile ofchildren with ADHD rather than ASD,who tend to have marked elevationson the Shift scale. In Adam’s report,we might write: The profile pattern islike that seen in children diagnosedwith ADHD. n Adam’s motherStandardizationluADHDASD80T scores7570lu65 uu60 ulln lnnnnInhibitShiftEmotional ControlWorking MemoryPlan/Organize5550 ul45BRIEF-P scalesFigure 2. Adam’s BRIEF-P Parent and Teacher Form scale T scores plotted against mean scale T scores forvarious clinical groups. ADHD attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; ASD autism spectrum disorder.8

For example, a child may show better flexibility or adaptability at home than in school or vice versa, and this cansuggest ways to import supports that are helpful from oneenvironment into the other. In order to facilitate inter pretation across raters, it can be helpful to examinedifferences between raters’ T scores and the base rates ofthe differences and to consider interrater reliabilities (seeAppendix C).difference between specific scores is. The lower thepercentage, the more uncommon the difference. Uncom mon discrepancies between raters should be investigatedto determine why they exist. As seen in Table C.2, approximately 60% of rater pairs are within 10 T-score points ofeach other, with an additional 15% within 10 to 20 T-scorepoints, resulting in the majority of rater pairs being within20 T-score points of each other. Thus, it is unusual to haveratings that are 20 or more T-score points apart. As ageneral rule, differences between raters of more than 10T-score points might suggest very different perspectivesthat warrants further exploration. As shown in Figure 3,the largest difference between Adam’s mother’s and histeacher’s ratings was found on the Plan/Organize scale.This difference was also relatively uncommon, occurring inonly 14.3% of the sample, indicating that Adam’s motherand his teacher disagreed about the severity of his problems more than is typical, though the difference was notstatistically significant. In Adam’s report, we might simplywrite: Parent and teacher ratings were in good agreement.To facilitate clinical interpretation of differences betweenparent and teacher ratings, reliable change index (RCI)scores are provided (see Table C.1). The T-score valuesrequired to indicate a significant difference between parentand teacher BRIEF-P scores are listed for each scale andindex at the 80% (p .20), 90% (p .10), 95% (p .05), and 99% (p .01) confidence levels. In the clinicalsetting, a T-score difference that exceeds the 80% confidence level is usually considered meaningful. To interpretthe significance of the difference between two scores ofthe same scale or index, calculate the absolute differencebetween the two scores and compare with the values inTable C.1. Figure 3 shows Adam’s parent and teacherT-score differences for each scale and index and thesignificance levels. For example, Adam’s mother’s ratingson the Inhibit scale resulted in a T score of 86, and histeacher ratings on the same scale resulted in a T score of84 for an absolute difference of 2. Table C.1 shows thatthis difference is not significant.Putting It All TogetherWhile these interpretive steps may seem cumbersomeat first, they can result in a more thorough and nuancedinterpretation of BRIEF-P profiles. In this case, following thesteps in Table 2 would result in an interpretive paragraphsimilar to the following:Parent and teacher ratings of Adam’s everyday executivefunctioning were in good agreement and indicated markedproblems inhibiting impulses, sustaining working memory andattention, and planning and organizing problem solving.Parent ratings also indicated marked problems regulatingIn addition to considering the significance of T-scoredifferences between raters, the percentages of T-scoredifferences derived from the interrater sample should bereviewed to determine how common the absoluteScale/index/compositeBRIEF-PParentT scoreBRIEF-PTeacherT scoreAbsolutedifferenceSignificancelevel% l Control72639ns58.9Working ry Self-Control Index (ISCI)83794ns61.1Flexibility Index (FI)65605ns62.9Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI)75827ns64.0Global Executive Composite (GEC)79801ns58.4Figure 3. BRIEF-P Parent and Teacher score discrepancies for Adam. ns not significant.9

emotions. Elevations of this magnitude (T 65) on the BRIEF-P Inhibit,Working Memory, and Plan/Organize scales are seen in less than 10% oftypically developing children but are commonly seen in children diagnosedwith attention disorders. The elevation on the Emotional Control scale isalso seen in less than 10% of typically developing children but in approximately 50% of children with ADHD. Adam’s profile of strengths andweaknesses in self-regulation is similar to students diagnosed with ADHD.Recommendations for AdamWith this BRIEF-P profile, Adam is at high risk for continued behavioral, social, and emotional difficulties that will likely interfere with hissuccess across multiple domains. Recommendations should focus onbolstering inhibitory control as the primary need. Because of theextreme nature of his difficulties and their effect on his functioning,Adam may be referred for pharmacological consultation. Because Adamis too young and too impulsive to consider consequences with anydelay, he and his family should be referred to a behavioral specialistwho can design a program focused on controlling antecedents to hisimpulsive behaviors. At the same time, consequences—as long as theyare meaningful, consistent, and immediate—could be helpful in supporting better inhibitory control and better social interactions. Minimal focuswas given to working memory and metacognitive aspects of executivefunction because inhibitory control needs to improve first.10Following these steps offersa more comprehensiveevaluation of the child’sself-regulatory functioningacross school and homeenvironments.

ReferencesAdams, J. N., Feldman, H. M., Huffman, L. C., & Loe, I. M.(2015). Sensory processing in preterm preschoolersand its association with executive function. Early HumanDevelopment, 91, 227-233.Edgin, J. O., Tooley, U., Demara, B., Nyhuis, C., Anand, P., &Spanò, G. (2015). Sleep disturbance and expressivelanguage development in preschool-age children withDown syndrome. Child Development, 86, 1984-1998.Alduncin, N., Huffman, L. C., Feldman, H. M., & Loe, I. M.(2014). Executive function is associated with socialcompetence in preschool-aged children born preterm orfull term. Early Human Development, 90, 299-306.Etemad, P. (2011). The relationship of everyday executivefunction and autism spectrum disorder symptoms inpreschoolers. Dissertation Abstracts International SectionA, 71, 3224.Anderson, S. E., McNamara, K., Andridge, R., & Keim, S. A.(2015). Executive function and mealtime behavioramong preschool-aged children born very preterm.Eating Behaviors, 19, 110-114.Ezpeleta, L., & Granero, R. (2015). Executive functions inpreschoolers with ADHD, ODD, and comorbid ADHDODD: Evidence from ecological and performance-basedmeasures. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9, 258-270.Baron, I. S., Erickson, K., Ahronovich, M. D., Baker, R., &Litman, F. R. (2011). Neuropsychological and behavioraloutcomes of extremely low birth weight at age three.Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 5-21.Gioia, G., Espy, K., & Isquith, P. (2003) Behavior RatingInventory of Executive Function–Preschool Version. Lutz,FL: PAR.Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2015).Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, SecondEdition. Lutz, FL: PAR.Baron, I. S., Weiss, B. A., Baker, R., Khoury, A., Remsburg,I., Thermolice, J. W., & .Ahronovich, M. D. (2014).Subtle adverse effects of late preterm birth: A cautionarynote. Neuropsychology, 28, 11-18.Holt, R. F., Beer, J., Kronenberger, W. G., Pisoni, D. B., &Lalonde, K. (2012). Contribution of family environmentto pediatric cochlear implant users’ speech and lan gu age outcomes: Some preliminary findings. Journal ofSpeech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 848-864.Crowe, L. M., Catroppa, C., Babl, F. E., & Anderson, V.(2013). Executive function outcomes of children withtraumatic brain injury sustained before three years. ChildNeuropsychology, 19, 113-126.Isquith, P., Gioia, G., Guy, S., & Kenworthy, L. (2015).Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, SecondEdition Interpretive Guide. Lutz, FL: PAR.d’Ardhuy, X. L., Edgin, J. O., Bouis, C., de Sola, S., Goeldner,C., Kishnani, P., & . Khwaja, O. (2015). Assessment ofcognitive scales to examine memory, executive functionand language in individuals with Down syndrome:Implications of a 6-month observational study. FrontiersIn Behavioral Neuroscience, 9.Jahromi, L. B., Bryce, C. I., & Swanson, J. (2013). The importance of self-regulation for the school and peer engagement of children with high-functioning autism. Researchin Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 235-246.Daunhauer, L. A., Fidler, D. J., Hahn, L., Will, E., Lee, N. R.,& Hepburn, S. (2014). Profiles of everyday executivefunctioning in young children with Down syndrome.American Journal on Intellectual and DevelopmentalDisabilities, 119, 303-318.Karver, C. L., Wade, S. L., Cassedy, A., Taylor, H. G., Stancin,T., Yeates, K. O., & Walz, N. C. (2012). Age at injuryand long-term behavior problems after traumatic braininjury in young children. Rehabilitation Psychology, 57,256-265Daunhauer, L. A., Gerlach-McDonald, B., Will, E., & Fidler,D. J. (2017). Performance and ratings based measuresof executive function in school-aged children withDown syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 42,351-368.Lee, N. R., Fidler, D. J., Blakeley-Smith, A., Daunhauer, L.,Robinson, C., & Hepburn, S. L. (2011). Caregiver reportof executive functioning in a population-based sampleof young children with Down syndrome. American Jour nal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116,290-304.11

Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2016). The effect of bilingualexposure on executive function skills in preterm and fullterm preschoolers. Journal of Developmental and Behav ioral Pediatrics, 37, 548-556.Schneider, H. E., Lam, J. C., & Mahone, E. M. (2016). Sleepdisturbance and neuropsychological function in youngchild ren with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, 22, 493-506.Skogan, A. H., Zeiner, P. å., Egeland, J., Urnes, A. G., ReichbornKjennerud, T., & Aase, H. (2015). Parent ratings of executive function in young preschool children with symptomsof attention-deficit/-hyperactivity disorder. Behavioral andBrain

Strategies for interpreting the BRIEF-P scales are provided in the BRIEF-P Profes-sional Manual. The following section describes an enhanced interpretation approach as outlined in Table 2. Table 2 provides the key steps for interpreting the BRIEF-P and includes associated references and examples of statements that might be

Related Documents:

Opti Tri White Par Introduction / Elation Professional - www.elationlighting.com - Opti Tri White Par Instruction Manual Page 2 Unpacking: Thank you for purchasing the Opti Tri White Par by Ela-tion Professional . Every Opti Tri White Par has been thoroughly tested and has been shipped in perfect operating condition. Carefully check

Intelligence tests and personality tests are indeed psychological tests—and they are indeed used to identify giftedness and diagnose psychological disorders. However, this is only a snapshot of what psychological testing is all about. There are many types of psychological tests, and they have many different purposes.

Psychological Research Methods Excavating Human Behaviors. 2 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science Chapter 1. 3 Thinking Critically with Psychological Science The Need for Psychological Science The limits of Intuition and Comm

4.302 Psychological Testing Adolescent/Child 6.400 Outpatient Treatment 6.403 Psychological Testing The Psychological Evaluation Request (PER) form for pre-authorization of neuropsychological or psychological testing, as well as a list of psychological tests, is

Apr 17, 2012 · Sysco South Florida Product Guide 5113295 500/EA SYSCO Bag Paper White 25 Lb 5113386 6/500 CT SYSCO Bag Paper White 2 Lb 5113378 4/500 CT SYSCO Bag Paper White 4lb 5113352 2/500 CT SYSCO Bag Paper White 6 Lb 5113345 2/500 CT SYSCO Bag Paper White 8 Lb 0047011 200/CT DURO Bag Papr Brn Hdl Meals To Go 6098834 1/10 CT AUGTHOM Bag Pastry

CAPE Management of Business Specimen Papers: Unit 1 Paper 01 60 Unit 1 Paper 02 68 Unit 1 Paper 03/2 74 Unit 2 Paper 01 78 Unit 2 Paper 02 86 Unit 2 Paper 03/2 90 CAPE Management of Business Mark Schemes: Unit 1 Paper 01 93 Unit 1 Paper 02 95 Unit 1 Paper 03/2 110 Unit 2 Paper 01 117 Unit 2 Paper 02 119 Unit 2 Paper 03/2 134

The Project Brief can take two forms: A letter Brief may be used for projects less than 100,000 (total cost including GST and fees). Full Brief utilising a project specific brief with this Basic Brief. The Project Brief in its dra

Black Holes and the expansion of the Universe (covered in Physics 162) are two subjects that rest completely on Einstein’s General Relativity. We will not be able to cover GR in depth, but we will understand the essential concepts at a level even most PhD physicists do not. And we will do actual calculations of what happens around and even inside black holes. Most physicists don’t study GR .