The Shame Of American Education

3y ago
17 Views
2 Downloads
831.50 KB
8 Pages
Last View : 5m ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Brady Himes
Transcription

The Shame of American EducationB. F. SkinnerABSTRACT: Recent analyses of American schoolsand proposals for school reform have missed anessential point: Most current problems could besolved if students learned twice as much in the sametime and with the same effort. It has been shownthat they can do so (a) when the goals of educationare clarified, (b) when each student is permitted toadvance at his or her own pace, and (c) when theproblem of motivation is solved with programmedinstructional materials, so designed that students arevery often right and learn at once that they are. Thetheories of human behavior most often taught inschools of education stand in the way of this solutionto the problem of American education, but the proposal that schools of education simply be disbandedis a step in the wrong direction. Teachers need to betaught how to teach, and a technology is now availablethat will permit them to teach much more effectively.On a morning in October 1957, Americans wereawakened by the beeping of a satellite. It was aRussian satellite, Sputnik. Why was it not American?Was something wrong with American education?Evidently so, and money was quickly voted toimprove American schools, Now we are being awakened by the beepings of Japanese cars, Japaneseradios, phonographs, and television sets, and Japanese wristwatch alarms, and again questions arebeing asked about American education, especiallyin science and mathematics.Something does seem to be wrong. Accordingto a-recent report of the National Commission onExcellence in Education (1983), for example, theaverage achievement of our high-school students onstandardized tests is now lower than it was a quarterof a century ago, and students in American schoolscompare poorly with those in other nations in manyfields. As the commission put it, America is threatened by "a rising tide of mediocrity."The first wave of reform is usually rhetorical.To improve education we are said to need "imaginative innovations," a "broad national effort" leadingto a "deep and lasting change," and a "commitmentto excellence." More specific suggestions have beenmade, however. To get better teachers we should paythem more, possibly according to merit. They shouldbe certified to teach the subjects they teach. To getbetter students, scholarship standards should beSeptember 1984 American PsychologistCopyright 1984 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.Vol. 39, No. 9, 947-954Harvard Universityraised. The school day should be extended from 6to 7 hours, more time should be spent on homework,and the school year should be lengthened from 180to 200, or even 220, days. We should change whatwe are teaching. Social studies are all very well, butthey should not take time away from basics, especiallymathematics.As many of us have learned to expect, there isa curious omission in that list: It contains nosuggestion that teaching be improved. There is aconspiracy of silence about teaching as a skill. TheNew York Times publishes a quarterly survey ofeducation. Three recent issues (Fisk, 1982, 1983a,1983b) contained 18 articles about the kinds ofthings being taught in schools; 11 articles about thefinancial problems of students and schools; 10 articlesabout the needs of special students, from the giftedto the disadvantaged; and smaller numbers of articlesabout the selection of students, professional problemsof teachers, and sports and other extracurricularactivities. Of about 70 articles, only 2 had anythingto do with how students are taught or how theycould be taught better. Pedagogy is a dirty word.In January 1981, Frederick Mosteller, presidentof the American Association for the Advancementof Science, gave an address called "Innovation andEvaluation" (Mosteller, 1981). He began with anexample of the time which can pass between ascientific discovery and its practical use. The factthat lemon juice cures scurvy was discovered in1601, but more than 190 years passed before theBritish navy began to use citrus juice on a regularbasis and another 70 before scurvy was wiped outin the mercantile marine—a lag of 264 years. Lagshave grown shorter but, as Mosteller pointed out,are often still too long. Perhaps unwittingly he gaveanother example. He called for initiatives in scienceand engineering education and said that a majortheme of the 1982 meeting of the association wouldbe a "national commitment to educational excellencein science and engineering for all Americans"(p. 886),When Mosteller's address was published inScience, I wrote a letter to the editor (Skinner, 1981)calling attention to an experiment in teaching algebrain a school in Roanoke, Virginia (Rushton, 1965).In this experiment an eighth-grade class using simpleteaching machines and hastily composed instructional programs went through all of ninth-grade947

algebra in half a year. Their grades met ninth-grade kept. But that should not have concealed the valuenorms, and when tested a year later the students of programmed instruction for so many years. Thereremembered rather more than usual. Had American is more than that to be said for the marketplace ineducators decided that that was the way to teach the selection of a better mousetrap.algebra? They had not. The experiment was donein 1960, but education had not yet made any use Psychological Roadblocksof it. The lag was already 21 years long.I shall argue that educators have not seized thisA month or so later I ran into Mosteller. "Did chance to solve their problems because the solutionyou see my letter in Science about teaching ma- conflicts with deeply entrenched views of humanchines?" I asked. "Teaching machines?" he said, behavior, and that these views are too stronglypuzzled. "Oh, you mean computers—teaching ma- supported by current psychology. Humanistic psychines to you." And, of course, he was right. Com- chologists, for example, tend to feel threatened byputer is the current word. But is it the right one? any kind of scientific analysis of human behavior,Computers are now badly misnamed. They were particularly if it leads to a "technology" that can bedesigned to compute, but they are not computing used to intervene in people's lives. A technology ofwhen they are processing words, or displaying Pac- teaching is especially threatening. Carl Rogers hasMan, or aiding instruction (unless the instruction is said that teaching is vastly overrated, and Ivan Illichin computing). "Computer" has all the respectability has called for the de-schooling of society. I dealtof the white-collar executive, whereas "machine" is .with the problem in Beyond Freedom and Dignitydefinitely blue-collar, but let us call things by their (Skinner, 1971). To give a single example, we do notright names. Instruction may be "computer aided," like to be told something we already know, forand all good instruction must be "interactive," but we can then no longer claim credit for havingmachines that teach are teaching machines.known it.I liked the Roanoke experiment because itTo solve that problem, Plato tried to show thatconfirmed something I had said a few years earlier students already possess knowledge and have onlyto the effect that with teaching machines and pro- to be shown that they possess it. But the famousgrammed instruction one could teach what is now scene in Plato's Meno in which Socrates shows thattaught in American schools in half the time with the slaveboy already knows Pythagoras's theoremhalf the effort. I shall not review other evidence that for doubling the square is one of the great intellectualthat is true. Instead I shall demonstrate my faith in hoaxes of all time. The slaveboy agrees with everya technology of teaching by going out on a limb. I thing Socrates says, but there is no evidence whatclaim that the school system of any large American soever that he could then go through the proof bycity could be so redesigned, at little or no additional himself. Indeed, Socrates says that the boy wouldcost, that students would come to school and apply need to be taken through it many times before hethemselves to their work with a minimum of punitive could do so.coercion and, with very rare exceptions, learn toCognitive psychology is causing much moreread with reasonable ease, express themselves well trouble, but in a different way. It is hard to bein speech and writing, and solve a fair range of precise because the field is usually presented in whatmathematical problems. I want to talk about why we may call a cognitive style. For example, a pamthis has not been done.phlet of the National Institute of Education (1980)The teaching machines of 25 years ago were quotes with approval the contention that "at thecrude, of course, but that is scarcely an explanation. present time, modern cognitive psychology is theThe calculating machines were crude, too, yet they dominant theoretical force in psychological sciencewere used until they could be replaced by something as opposed to the first half of the century whenbetter. The hardware problem has now been solved, behavioristic, anti-mentalistic stimulus-response thebut resistance to a technology of teaching survives. ories of learning were in the ascendance" (p. 391).The rank commercialism which quickly engulfed (The writer means "ascendant.") The pamphlet tellsthe field of teaching machines is another possible us that cognitive science studies learning, but not inexplanation. Too many people rushed in to write quite those words. Instead, cognitive science is saidbad programs and make promises that could not be to be "characterized by a concern with understandingthe mechanisms by which human beings carry outcomplex intellectual activities including learning"An earlier version of this article was given as the Bode Lecture(p. 391). The pamphlet also says that cognitiveat Ohio State University, April 8, 1981.sciencecan help construct tests that will tell us moreRequests for reprints should be sent to B. F. Skinner,aboutwhata student has learned and hence how toHarvard University, Department of Psychology and Social Relateach better, but here is the way it says this: "Attentions, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138.tion will be placed on two specific topics: Applications948September 1984 American Psychologist

of cognitive models of the knowledge structure ofvarious subject matters and of learning and problemsolving to construction of tests that identify processesunderlying test answers, analyze errors, and provideinformation about what students know and don'tknow, and strategies for integrating testing information with instructional decisions" (p. 393). Noticeespecially the cognitive style in the last phrase—thequestion is not "whether test results can suggestbetter ways of teaching" but "whether there arestrategies for integrating testing information withinstructional decisions."The Commission on Behavioral and SocialSciences and Education of the National ResearchCouncil (1984) provides a more recent example inits announcement of a biennial program plan covering the period 1 May 1983 to 30 April 1985. Thecommission will take advantage of "significant advances . . . in the cognitive sciences" (p. 41). Willit study learning? Well, not exactly. The memberswill "direct their attention to studies of fundamentalprocesses underlying the nature and development oflearning" (p. 41). Why do cognitive psychologistsnot tell us frankly what they are up to? Is it possiblethat they themselves do not really know?Cognitive psychology is certainly in the ascendant. The word cognitive is sprinkled through thepsychological literature like salt—and, like salt, notso much for any flavor of its own but to bring outthe flavor of other things, things which a quarter ofa century ago would have been called by othernames. The heading of an article in a recent issueof the APA Monitor (Turkington, 1983) tells us that"cognitive deficits" are important in understandingalcoholism. In the text we leam simply that alcoholicsshow losses in perception and motor skills. Perceptionand motor skills used to be fields of psychology;now they are fields of cognitive science. Nothing hasbeen changed except the name, and the change hasbeen made for suspicious reasons. There is a senseof profundity about "cognitive deficits," but it doesnot take us any deeper into the subject.Much of the vogue of cognitive science is dueto advances in computer technology. The computeroffers an appealing simplification of some old psychological problems. Sensation and perception arereduced to input; learning and memory to theprocessing, storage, and retrieval of information;and action to output. It is very much like the oldstimulus-response formula patched up with intervening variables. To say that students process information is to use a doubtful metaphor, and how theyprocess information is still the old question of howthey learn.Cognitive psychology also gains prestige fromits alignment with brain research. Interesting thingsare certainly being discovered about the biochemistrySeptember 1984 American Psychologistand circuitry of the brain, but we are still a longway from knowing what is happening in the brainas behavior is shaped and maintained by contingencies of reinforcement, and that means that we are along way from help in designing useful instructionalpractices,Cognitive science is also said to be supportedby modern linguistics, a topic to which I am particularly sensitive. Programmed instruction emergedfrom my analysis of verbal behavior (Skinner, 1957),which linguists, particularly generative grammarians,have, of course, attacked. So far as I know they haveoffered no equally effective practices. One mightexpect them to have improved the teaching oflanguages, but almost all language laboratories stillwork in particularly outmoded ways, and languageinstruction is one of the principal failures of precollege education.Psycholinguistics moves in essentially the samedirection in its hopeless commitment to development.Behavior is said to change in ways determined byits structure. The change may be a function of age,but age is not a variable that one can manipulate.The extent to which developmentalism has encouraged a neglect of more useful ways of changingbehavior is shown by a recent report (Siegler, 1983)in which the number of studies concerned with thedevelopment of behavior in children was found tohave skyrocketed, whereas the number concernedwith how children learn has dropped to a point atwhich the researcher could scarcely find any examples at all.There are many fine cognitive psychologistswho are doing fine research, but they are not thecognitive psychologists who for 25 years have beenpromising great advances in education. A shortpaper published in Science last April (Resnick, 1983)asserts that "recent findings in cognitive sciencesuggest new approaches to teaching in science andmathematics" (p. 477), but the examples given,when expressed in noncognitive style, are simplythese: (a) Students learn about the world in "naive"ways before they study science; (b) naive theoriesinterfere with learning scientific theories; (c) weshould therefore teach science as early as possible;(d) many problems are not solved exclusively withmathematics; qualitative experience is important;(e) students learn more than isolated facts; theylearn how facts are related to each other; and (f)students relate what they are learning to what theyalready know. If these are recent findings, where hascognitive science been?Cognitive psychology is frequently presented asa revolt against behaviorism, but it is not a revolt;it is a retreat. Everyday English is full of termsderived from ancient explanations of human behavior. We spoke that language when we were young.949

When we went out into the world and becamepsychologists, we learned to speak in other ways butmade mistakes for which we were punished. Butnow we can relax. Cognitive psychology is OldHome Week. We are back among friends speakingthe language we spoke when we were growing up.We can talk about love and will and ideas andmemories and feelings and states of mind, and noone will ask us what we mean; no one will raise aneyebrow.Schools of EducationPsychological theories come into the hands of teachers through schools of education and teachers' colleges, and it is there, I think, that we must lay themajor blame for what is happening in Americaneducation. In a recent article in the New York Times(Botstein, 1983), President Leon Botstein of BardCollege proposed that schools of education, teachers'colleges, and departments of education simply bedisbanded. But he gave a different reason. He saidthat schools of that sort "placed too great an emphasis on pedagogical techniques and psychologicalstudies" (p. 64), when they should be teaching thesubjects the teachers will eventually teach. But disbanding such schools is certainly a move in thewrong direction. It has long been said that collegeteaching is the only profession for which there is noprofessional training. Would-be doctors go to medicalschools, would-be lawyers go to law schools, andwould-be engineers go to institutes of technology,but would-be college teachers just start teaching.Fortunately it is recognized that grade- and highschool teachers need to learn to teach. The troubleis, they are not being taught in effective ways. Thecommitment to humanistic and cognitive psychologyis only part of the problem.Equally damaging is the assumption that teaching can be adequately discussed in everyday English.The appeal to laymanship is attractive. At the "Convocation on Science and Mathematics, in theSchools" called by the National Academies of Sciences and Engineering, one member said that "whatwe need are bright, energetic, dedicated young people,trained in mathematics . . . science . . . or technology, mixing it up with 6- to 13-year-old kids inthe classroom" (Raizen, 1983, p. 19). The problemis too grave to be solved in any such way. The firstpage of the report notes with approval that "if thereis one American enterprise that is local in its designand control it is education" (p. 1). That is held tobe a virtue. But certainly the commission would notapprove similar statements about medicine, law, orscience and technology. Why should the communitydecide how children are to be taught? The commission is actually pointing to one explanation of whyeducation is failing.950We must beware of the fallacy of the goodteacher and the good student. There are many goodteachers who have not needed to learn to teach.They would be good at almost anything they tried.There are many good students who scarcely need tobe taught. Put a good teacher and a good studenttogether and you have what seems to be an idealinstructional setting. But it is disastrous to take itas a model to be followed in our schools, wherehundreds of thousands of teachers must teach millions of students. Teachers must learn how to teach,and they must be taught by schools of education.They need only to be taught more effective ways ofteaching.A SolutionWe could solve our major problems in education ifstudents learned more during each day in school.That does not mean a longer day or year or morehomework. It simply means using time more efficiently. Such a solution is not considered in any ofthe reports I have mentioned—whether from theNational Institute of Education, the American Association for the Advancement of Science, the National Research Council, or the National Academiesof Sciences and Engineering. Nevertheless, it is withineasy reach. Here is all that needs to be done.1. Be clear about what is to be taught. WhenI once explained to a group of grade-school teachershow I would teach children to spell words, one ofthem said, "Yes, but can you teach spelling?" Forhim, students spelled words correctly not becausethey had learned to do so but because they hadacquired a special ability. When I told a physicistcolleague about the Roanoke experiment

Harvard University, Department of Psychology and Social Rela-tions, William James Hall, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, Mas-sachusetts 02138. kept. But that should not have concealed the value of programmed instruction for so many years. There is more than that to be said for the marketplace in the selection of a better mousetrap. Psychological .

Related Documents:

Shame and humiliation 16 Shame and self esteem 17 Shame and narcissism 18 II) The Difference Between Shame and Guilt 20 Attribution theory 22 III) Empathy 23 Empathy and sympathy 25 Empathy, personal distress and shame 25 IV) Shame Guilt and Empathy 27 Theories concerning shame and empathy 34 The measurement of shame guilt and empathy 36 V) Sex .

May 02, 2018 · D. Program Evaluation ͟The organization has provided a description of the framework for how each program will be evaluated. The framework should include all the elements below: ͟The evaluation methods are cost-effective for the organization ͟Quantitative and qualitative data is being collected (at Basics tier, data collection must have begun)

Silat is a combative art of self-defense and survival rooted from Matay archipelago. It was traced at thé early of Langkasuka Kingdom (2nd century CE) till thé reign of Melaka (Malaysia) Sultanate era (13th century). Silat has now evolved to become part of social culture and tradition with thé appearance of a fine physical and spiritual .

On an exceptional basis, Member States may request UNESCO to provide thé candidates with access to thé platform so they can complète thé form by themselves. Thèse requests must be addressed to esd rize unesco. or by 15 A ril 2021 UNESCO will provide thé nomineewith accessto thé platform via their émail address.

̶The leading indicator of employee engagement is based on the quality of the relationship between employee and supervisor Empower your managers! ̶Help them understand the impact on the organization ̶Share important changes, plan options, tasks, and deadlines ̶Provide key messages and talking points ̶Prepare them to answer employee questions

Dr. Sunita Bharatwal** Dr. Pawan Garga*** Abstract Customer satisfaction is derived from thè functionalities and values, a product or Service can provide. The current study aims to segregate thè dimensions of ordine Service quality and gather insights on its impact on web shopping. The trends of purchases have

Chính Văn.- Còn đức Thế tôn thì tuệ giác cực kỳ trong sạch 8: hiện hành bất nhị 9, đạt đến vô tướng 10, đứng vào chỗ đứng của các đức Thế tôn 11, thể hiện tính bình đẳng của các Ngài, đến chỗ không còn chướng ngại 12, giáo pháp không thể khuynh đảo, tâm thức không bị cản trở, cái được

Jan 06, 2019 · The first element of shame resilience is recognizing shame and understanding our triggers. Men . and women who are resilient to shame have this capacity. This enables them to respond to shame with awareness and understanding. When we can’t recognize shame and underst