Cur Rent Issues In The T Eaching Of Grammar: An SLA .

2y ago
46 Views
2 Downloads
204.37 KB
25 Pages
Last View : 15d ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Hayden Brunner
Transcription

Current Issues in the Teaching ofGrammar: An SLA PerspectiveROD ELLISUniversity of AucklandAuckland, New ZealandThe study of how learners acquire a second language (SLA) has helpedto shape thinking about how to teach the grammar of a secondlanguage. There remain, however, a number of controversial issues.This paper considers eight key questions relating to grammar pedagogyin the light of findings from SLA. As such, this article complementsCelce-Murcia’s (1991) article on grammar teaching in the 25th anniversary issue of TESOL Quarterly, which considered the role of grammar ina communicative curriculum and drew predominantly on a linguistictheory of grammar. These eight questions address whether grammarshould be taught and if so what grammar, when, and how. AlthoughSLA does not afford definitive solutions to these questions, it serves thevaluable purpose of problematising this aspect of language pedagogy.This article concludes with a statement of my own beliefs aboutgrammar teaching, grounded in my own understanding of SLA.This article identifies and discusses a number of key issues relating tothe teaching of grammar in a second language (L2) and, by drawingon theory and research in SLA, suggests ways to address these problems.It points to a number of alternative solutions to each problem, indicatingthat more often than not there are no clear solutions currently available.The aim, therefore, is not to identify new solutions to existing controversies, nor even to present new controversies. Rather it addresses withinthe compass of a single article a whole range of issues related to grammarteaching, problematises these issues, and by so doing, provides a counterweight to the advocacy of specific, but also quite limited, proposals forteaching grammar that have originated in some SLA quarters. However,I conclude with a statement of my own position on these issues.The questions that will be addressed are1. Should we teach grammar, or should we simply create the conditionsby which learners learn naturally?2. What grammar should we teach?TESOL QUARTERLY Vol. 40, No. 1, March 200683

3. When should we teach grammar? Is it best to teach grammar whenlearners first start to learn an L2 or to wait until later when learnershave already acquired some linguistic competence?4. Should grammar instruction be massed (i.e., the available teachingtime be concentrated into a short period) or distributed (i.e., theavailable teaching time spread over a longer period)?5. Should grammar instruction be intensive (e.g., cover a single grammatical structure in a single lesson) or extensive (e.g., cover manygrammatical structures in a single lesson)?6. Is there any value in teaching explicit grammatical knowledge?7. Is there a best way to teach grammar for implicit knowledge?8. Should grammar be taught in separate lessons or integrated intocommunicative activities?DEFINING GRAMMAR TEACHINGTraditionally, grammar teaching is viewed as the presentation andpractice of discrete grammatical structures. This is the view promulgatedin teacher handbooks. Ur (1996), for example, in her chapter titled“Teaching Grammar” has sections on “presenting and explaining grammar” and “grammar practice activities.” Hedge (2000) in her chaptertitled “Grammar” similarly only considers “presenting grammar” and“practising grammar.” This constitutes an overly narrow definition ofgrammar teaching. It is certainly true that grammar teaching can consistof the presentation and practice of grammatical items. But, as willbecome apparent, it need not. First, some grammar lessons might consistof presentation by itself (i.e., without any practice), while others mightentail only practice (i.e., no presentation). Second, grammar teachingcan involve learners in discovering grammatical rules for themselves(i.e., no presentation and no practice). Third, grammar teaching can beconducted simply by exposing learners to input contrived to providemultiple exemplars of the target structure. Here, too, there is nopresentation and no practice, at least in the sense of eliciting productionof the structure. Finally, grammar teaching can be conducted by meansof corrective feedback on learner errors when these arise in the contextof performing some communicative task. The definition of grammarteaching that informs this article is a broad one:Grammar teaching involves any instructional technique that draws learners’attention to some specific grammatical form in such a way that it helps themeither to understand it metalinguistically and/or process it in comprehensionand/or production so that they can internalize it.84TESOL QUARTERLY

SHOULD WE TEACH GRAMMAR?This question was motivated by early research into naturalistic L2acquisition, which showed that learners appeared to follow a naturalorder and sequence of acquisition (i.e., they mastered different grammatical structures in a relatively fixed and universal order and theypassed through a sequence of stages of acquisition on route to masteringeach grammatical structure). This led researchers like Corder (1967) tosuggest that learners had their own built-in syllabus for learning grammar. In line with this, Krashen (1981) argued that grammar instructionplayed no role in acquisition, a view based on the conviction thatlearners (including classroom learners) would automatically proceedalong their built-in syllabus as long as they had access to comprehensibleinput and were sufficiently motivated. Grammar instruction could contribute to learning but this was of limited value because communicativeability was dependent on acquisition.There followed a number of empirical studies designed to (a)compare the order of acquisition of instructed and naturalistic learners(e.g., Pica, 1983), (b) compare the success of instructed and naturalisticlearners (Long, 1983) and (c) examine whether attempts to teachspecific grammatical structures resulted in their acquisition (e.g., White,Spada, Lightbown, & Ranta, 1991). These studies showed that, by andlarge, the order of acquisition was the same for instructed and naturalistic learners (although there were some interesting differences1), thatinstructed learners generally achieved higher levels of grammaticalcompetence than naturalistic learners and that instruction was noguarantee that learners would acquire what they had been taught. Theseresults were interpreted as showing that the acquisitional processes ofinstructed and naturalistic learning were the same but that instructedlearners progressed more rapidly and achieved higher levels of proficiency. Thus, some researchers concluded (e.g., Long, 1988) thatteaching grammar was beneficial but that to be effective grammar had tobe taught in a way that was compatible with the natural processes ofacquisition.Subsequent research, such as Norris and Ortega’s (2000) metaanalysis of 49 studies, has borne out the overall effectiveness of grammarteaching. Further, there is evidence that, contrary to Krashen’s (1993)continued claims, instruction contributes to both acquired knowledge(see Ellis, 2002a) as well as learned knowledge. There is also increasing1For example, Pica (1983) notes that some structures (e.g., plural–s) were used moreaccurately by instructed learners and some (e.g., Verb–ing) by naturalistic learners. In otherstructures (e.g., articles) there was no difference.CURRENT ISSUES IN THE TEACHING OF GRAMMAR85

evidence that naturalistic learning in the classroom (as, e.g., in immersion programmes) does not typically result in high levels of grammaticalcompetence (Genesee, 1987). In short, there is now convincing indirectand direct evidence to support the teaching of grammar. Nevertheless,doubts remain about the nature of the research evidence. Many studies(including most of those reviewed by Norris and Ortega) measurelearning in terms of constrained constructed responses (e.g., fill in the blanks,sentence joining, or sentence transformation), which can be expected tofavour grammar teaching. There is only mixed evidence that instructionresults in learning when it is measured by means of free constructedresponses (e.g., communicative tasks). Also, it remains the case thatlearners do not always acquire what they have been taught and that forgrammar instruction to be effective it needs to take account of howlearners develop their interlanguages. As we will see, there is controversyregarding both how interlanguage development occurs and how instruction can facilitate this.WHAT GRAMMAR SHOULD WE TEACH?Assuming, then, that grammar teaching can contribute to interlanguagedevelopment, the next logical question concerns what grammar weshould teach. This question can be broken down into two separatequestions:1. What kind of grammar should we base teaching on?2. Which grammatical features should we teach?Linguistics affords a broad selection of grammatical models to choosefrom, including structural grammars, generative grammars (based on atheory of universal grammar), and functional grammars. Traditionallysyllabuses have been based on structural or descriptive grammars.Structural syllabuses traditionally emphasised the teaching of form overmeaning (e.g., Lado, 1970). Though the influence of structural grammars is still apparent today, modern syllabuses rightly give more attention to the functions performed by grammatical forms. Thus, forexample, less emphasis is placed on such aspects of grammar as sentencepatterns or tense paradigms and more on the meanings conveyed bydifferent grammatical forms in communication. Some attempt was oncemade to exploit the insights to be gleaned from generative theories ofgrammar (see, e.g., Bright, 1965), but in general, syllabus designers andteachers have not found such models useful and have preferred to relyon modern descriptive grammars, such as Celce-Murcia and Larsen-86TESOL QUARTERLY

Freeman’s (1999) Grammar Book. This resource is especially valuablebecause it not only provides a comprehensive, clear, and pedagogicallyexploitable description of English grammar but also identifies the kindsof errors that L2 learners are known to make with different grammaticalstructures. Such information is important because it helps to identifywhich structures and which aspects of a structure require specialattention. The Grammar Book is also ideal in that it presents informationnot only about linguistic form but also about the semantic and discoursalmeanings realised by particular forms. As VanPatten, Williams, and Rott(2004) emphasise, establishing connections between form and meaningis a fundamental aspect of language acquisition. Thus, any referencegrammar that fails to describe the form-meaning connections of thetarget language must necessarily be inadequate. In general, then, thechoice of which type of grammar to use as a basis for teaching is not amajor source of controversy; descriptive grammars that detail the formmeaning relationships of the language are ascendant.In contrast, the choice of which grammatical structures to teach iscontroversial. Two polar positions can be identified and various positionsin between. At one end of this continuum is Krashen’s minimalistposition. Krashen (1982) argues that grammar teaching should belimited to a few simple and portable rules such as 3rd person–s and pasttense–ed that can be used to monitor output from the acquired system.He bases his argument on the claim that most learners are only capableof learning such simple rules—that more complex rules are generallynot learnable or, if they are, are beyond students’ ability to apply throughmonitoring. Krashen’s claim, however, is not warranted. There is nowample evidence that many learners are capable of mastering a widerange of explicit grammar rules. Green and Hecht (1992), for example,found that university-level students of English in Germany were able toproduce clear explanations for 85% of the grammatical errors they wereasked to explain, while overall the learners in their study (who includedsecondary school students) managed satisfactory explanations for 46%of the errors. Macrory and Stone (2000) reported that British comprehensive school students had a fairly good explicit understanding of theperfect tense in French (e.g., they understood its function, they knewthat some verbs used avoir and some être, they were familiar with theforms required by different pronouns, and they were aware of the needfor a final accent on the past participle). Hu (2002) found that adultChinese learners of English demonstrated correct metalinguistic knowledge of prototypical rules of six English structures (e.g., for the definitearticle specific reference constituted the prototypical rule) but were lessclear about the peripheral rules for these structures (e.g., generic reference).At the other pole is the comprehensive position: Teach the whole ofCURRENT ISSUES IN THE TEACHING OF GRAMMAR87

the grammar of the target language.2 This is the position adopted bymany course book writers (e.g., Walter & Swan, 1990) or authors ofgrammar practice materials (e.g., Murphy, 1994). Such a position wouldalso seem unwarranted because learners are clearly capable of learning asubstantial amount of the L2 grammar without instruction and becausemost teaching contexts have limited time available for teaching grammarso some selection is needed.What then should selection be based on? The answer would seemobvious—the inherent learning difficulty of different grammatical structures. The problem arises in how to determine this. To begin with, it isnecessary to distinguish two different senses of learning difficulty. This canrefer to (a) the difficulty learners have in understanding a grammaticalfeature and (b) to the difficulty they have in internalising a grammaticalfeature so that they are able to use it accurately in communication. Thesetwo senses relate to the distinction between learning grammar as explicitknowledge and as implicit knowledge, which is discussed later. Clearly,what is difficult to learn as explicit knowledge and as implicit knowledgeis not the same. For example, most learners have no difficulty in graspingthe rule for English third person–s but they have enormous difficulty ininternalising this structure so they can use it accurately. These two sensesof learning difficulty have not always been clearly distinguished inlanguage pedagogy, with the result that even when the stated goal is thedevelopment of implicit knowledge, it is the anticipated difficultystudents will have in understanding a feature that guides the selectionand grading of grammatical structures. Third person–s, for example, istypically taught very early in a course.How then has learning difficulty been established? Traditionally,factors such as the frequency of specific structures in the input and theirutility to learners have been invoked (Mackey, 1976), but these factorswould seem to have more to do with use3 than with inherent cognitivedifficulty. Here I consider two approaches that have figured in attemptsto delineate cognitive difficulty.1. Teach those forms that differ from the learners’ first language (L1).2. Teach marked rather than unmarked forms.2Of course, it is not possible to specify the whole grammar of a language. Though thegrammar of a language may be determinate, descriptions of it are certainly not. The LongmanA Grammar of Contemporary English (Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech, & Svartvik, 1972) ran to 1081pages (excluding index and bibliography) but doubtlessly does not account for all the knownfacts of English grammar. Nevertheless, there is a recognized canon of English structures that,in the eyes of syllabus designers and textbook writers, constitutes the grammar of English.3Structures like English articles that are very frequent in the input can impose considerablelearning difficulty. Structures such as English conditionals may be very useful to learners but arealso difficult to learn.88TESOL QUARTERLY

The first approach was, of course, the one adopted in many earlystructural courses based on a contrastive analysis of the learner’s L1 andthe target language. Although the contrastive analysis hypothesis asinitially formulated is clearly not tenable (see Ellis, 1985, chapter 2), SLAresearchers still generally agree that learners transfer at least some of thefeatures of their L1 into the L2. For example, there is ample evidence(Trahey & White, 1993) to show that French learners of English produceerrors of the kind Mary kissed passionately John because French permits anadverb to be positioned between the verb and the direct object.Nevertheless, contrastive analysis does not constitute a sound basis forselecting grammatical structures. In many teaching contexts, the learners come from mixed language backgrounds where it would be impossible to use contrastive analysis to tailor grammar teaching to the entiregroup because the learners have different L1s. Also, we simply do not yetknow enough about when difference does and does not translate intolearning difficulty, and in some cases, learning difficulty arises evenwhere there is no difference.The second approach, however, is also problematic. Markedness hasbeen defined in terms of whether a grammatical structure is in somesense frequent, natural, and basic or infrequent, unnatural, and deviantfrom a regular pattern (Richards, Platt, & Weber, 1985). Thus, the use ofan infinitive without to following make, as in He made me follow him can beconsidered marked because make is one the few verbs in English thattakes this kind of complement and because this pattern occurs onlyinfrequently. The general idea is that we should teach the markedfeatures and leave the learners to learn the unmarked forms naturally bythemselves. The problem is that, as the definition suggests, markednessremains a somewhat opaque concept, so that it is often difficult to applywith the precision needed to determine which structures to teach.The selection of grammatical content, then, remains very problematic. One solution to the kinds of problems I have mentioned is to baseselection on the known errors produced by learners. In this respect, listsof common learner errors such as those available in Turton and Heaton’s(1996) Longman Dictionary of Common Errors and Swan and Smith’s (2001)Learner English: A Teacher’s Guide to Interference and Other Problems arehelpful.The problems of selection probably explain why grammatical syllabuses are so similar and have changed so little over the years; it is safer tofollow what has been done before. Of course the selection of what toteach will also depend on the learner’s stage of development. Theproblems that the learner’s stage of development involve are discussed insubsequent sections.CURRENT ISSUES IN THE TEACHING OF GRAMMAR89

WHEN SHOULD WE TEACH GRAMMAR?There are two competing answers to this question. According to thefirst, it is best to emphasise the teaching of grammar in the early stages ofL2 acquisition. According to the second, it is best to emphasise meaningfocused instruction to begin with and introduce grammar teaching later,when learners have already begun to form their interlanguages. I willbriefly consider the arguments for both positions.A key premise of behaviourist theories of language learning is that“error like sin needs to be avoided at all costs” (Brooks, 1960). Thispremise holds that once learners have formed incorrect habits, they willhave difficulty eradicating them and replacing them with correct habits.Thus, it is necessary to ensure that learners develop correct habits in thefirst place. This was one of the key premises of the audiolingual method(Lado, 1964). Other arguments can be advanced in favour of beginningto teach grammar early. The alternative to a form-focused approachemphasises meaning and message creation, as in task-based languageteaching (Skehan, 1998), but many teachers believe that beginning-levellearners cannot engage in meaning-centred activities because they lackthe neces

7. Is there a best way to teach grammar for implicit knowledge? 8. Should grammar be taught in separate lessons or integrated into communicative activities? DEFINING GRAMMAR TEACHING T raditionally , grammar teaching is viewed as the presentation and practice of disc

Related Documents:

May 02, 2018 · D. Program Evaluation ͟The organization has provided a description of the framework for how each program will be evaluated. The framework should include all the elements below: ͟The evaluation methods are cost-effective for the organization ͟Quantitative and qualitative data is being collected (at Basics tier, data collection must have begun)

Silat is a combative art of self-defense and survival rooted from Matay archipelago. It was traced at thé early of Langkasuka Kingdom (2nd century CE) till thé reign of Melaka (Malaysia) Sultanate era (13th century). Silat has now evolved to become part of social culture and tradition with thé appearance of a fine physical and spiritual .

On an exceptional basis, Member States may request UNESCO to provide thé candidates with access to thé platform so they can complète thé form by themselves. Thèse requests must be addressed to esd rize unesco. or by 15 A ril 2021 UNESCO will provide thé nomineewith accessto thé platform via their émail address.

̶The leading indicator of employee engagement is based on the quality of the relationship between employee and supervisor Empower your managers! ̶Help them understand the impact on the organization ̶Share important changes, plan options, tasks, and deadlines ̶Provide key messages and talking points ̶Prepare them to answer employee questions

Dr. Sunita Bharatwal** Dr. Pawan Garga*** Abstract Customer satisfaction is derived from thè functionalities and values, a product or Service can provide. The current study aims to segregate thè dimensions of ordine Service quality and gather insights on its impact on web shopping. The trends of purchases have

Chính Văn.- Còn đức Thế tôn thì tuệ giác cực kỳ trong sạch 8: hiện hành bất nhị 9, đạt đến vô tướng 10, đứng vào chỗ đứng của các đức Thế tôn 11, thể hiện tính bình đẳng của các Ngài, đến chỗ không còn chướng ngại 12, giáo pháp không thể khuynh đảo, tâm thức không bị cản trở, cái được

4. Determination of fair rent. 5. Increase in fair rent in what cases admissible. 6. Increase of rent in certain cases. 7. Landlord not to claim or receive any thing in excess of fair rent or agreed rent. 8. Right of tenant paying rent or advance to receipt. 9. Right of tenant to deposit rent in certain cases. 10. Eviction of tenants. 10-A.

R ated Armature Cur rent Ò IR A rm s 14.3 Armature Cur rent at Stall Ò IS A rm s 1 7.5 P eak Armature Cur rent at Stall Ò IP A rm s 45.5 Torque Constant Ñ K T N m A rm s 0.70 (lb in A rm s) 6.2 V oltage Constant P er Phase Ñ K E m V m in µ 1 24.3 Phase R esistance Ñ R 0.1 1 R ated P ower R ate Ò Q R k W s 74