Critical Thinking Training For Army Officers Volume Two: A Model Of .

1y ago
4 Views
2 Downloads
568.82 KB
106 Pages
Last View : 1m ago
Last Download : 3m ago
Upload by : Mika Lloyd
Transcription

U.S. Army Research Institutefor the Behavioral and Social SciencesResearch Report 1882CRITICAL THINKING TRAININGFOR ARMY OFFICERSVOLUME TWO:A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKINGSusan C. Fischer and V. Alan SpikerAnacapa Sciences, Inc.Sharon L. RiedelU.S. Army Research InstituteFebruary 2009Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited.

U.S. ArmyAReesearch Instituteefor thee Behavvioral annd Sociaal Scienccesectoratee of the DepartmDment of the ArmyyA DireDeputty Chief of Stafff, G1Authoriized and approvedafor distribbution:BARBAARA A. BLACK,BPPh.D.Researrch Program ManagerTraininng and Leeader DevvelopmenntMICHELLE SAMMS, Ph.D.DirectoorTechnical review bySU.S. Army Ressearch InstittuteRobert Solick,Gregoryy Ruark, U.SS. Army Reesearch InstituteSNOTICESDISTRIBBUTION: PrimaryPdisstribution off this Research Reporrt has beenn made by ARI.Please address correspondecence conccerning disstribution ofo reports to: U.S. ArmyAResearcch Institute for the Behhavioral andd Social Sciences, Attnn: DAPE-ARI-ZXM,2511 Jefferson Davvis Highwayy, Arlingtonn, Virginia 22202-392626FINAL DISPOSITIDON: This Research Report maay be destrooyed whenn it is no loongerneeded. Please doo not returnn it to the U.S.UArmy Research InstituteIforr the Behavviorales.and Soccial ScienceNOTE: The findinggs in this Reesearch Reeport are noot to be connstrued as ana officialDepartmment of the ArmyAposition, unless so designaated by otheer authorizeed documeents.

REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE1. REPORT DATE (dd-mm-yy)2. REPORT TYPEFebruary 20093. DATES COVERED (from. . . to)FinalJanuary 2004 – November 20064. TITLE AND SUBTITLE5a. CONTRACT OR GRANT NUMBERCritical Thinking Training for Army OfficersVolume Two: A Model of Critical ThinkingW74V8H-04-C-00075b. PROGRAM ELEMENT NUMBER6227856. AUTHOR(S)5c. PROJECT NUMBERSusan C. Fischer, V. Alan Spiker (Anacapa Sciences, Inc.), andSharon L. Riedel (U.S. Army Research Institute)A7905d. TASK NUMBER3335e. WORK UNIT NUMBER8. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER7. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES)Anacapa Sciences, Inc.301 E. Carrillo St.Santa Barbara, CA 93101U.S. Army Research InstituteFort Leavenworth Research Unit851 McClellan AveFort Leavenworth, KS 660279. SPONSORING/MONITORING AGENCY NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES)U.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences2511 Jefferson Davis HighwayArlington, VA 22202-3926ATTN: Fort Leavenworth Research Unit10. MONITOR ACRONYMARI11. MONITOR REPORT NUMBERResearch Report 188212. DISTRIBUTION/AVAILABILITY STATEMENTApproved for public release; distribution is unlimited.13. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTESSubject Matter POC and Contracting Officer’s Representative: Dr. Sharon Riedel14. ABSTRACT (Maximum 200 words): This report is the second of three volumes describing a multi-year research program todevelop and evaluate web based training in critical thinking for Army officers. The first volume presents an overview of theresearch effort that developed and validated a theoretical model for the training, selected and validated eight high impactcritical thinking skills for Army officers, and developed and evaluated the training course. This volume describes the results ofa literature review on critical thinking, a model of critical thinking that forms the theoretical basis for the training, andinvestigations that were conducted to validate the model. Volume Three describes a web-based prototype training system thattrains two critical thinking skills. Included in Volume Three are a description of the functional requirements, pedagogicalprinciples, course content, and evaluation of the training. A fourth report (Fischer, Spiker, & Riedel, 2008) describes anexpanded version of the training system that provides training for eight critical thinking skills for Army officers.15. SUBJECT TERMSCritical thinking, computer-based training, web-based training, critical thinking skillSECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF16. REPORT17. ABSTRACT18. THIS PAGEUnclassifiedUnclassifiedUnclassified19. LIMITATION OF 20. NUMBER OF 21. RESPONSIBLE PERSONABSTRACTPAGESUnlimitedi102Diane HadjiosifTechnical Publications Specialist703/602-8047

ii

Research Report 1882CRITICAL THINKING TRAININGFOR ARMY OFFICERSVOLUME TWO:A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKINGSusan C. Fischer and V. Alan SpikerAnacapa Sciences, Inc.Sharon L. RiedelU.S. Army Research InstituteFort Leavenworth Research UnitStanley M. Halpin, ChiefU.S. Army Research Institute for the Behavioral and Social Sciences2511 Jefferson Davis Highway, Arlington, Virginia 22202-3926February 2009Army Project Number622785.A790Personnel, Performanceand Training TechnologyApproved for public release; distribution is unlimited.iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThe multifaceted research on critical thinking reported in this volume could not have beencompleted without the contributions of many individuals. We are deeply indebted to each personwho has made this work possible. We would like to Dr. Stan Halpin, Chief of the U.S. ArmyResearch Institute (ARI) Fort Leavenworth Research Unit and other members of the ARI stafffor their support and review of our research.Several members of the Anacapa staff were significant contributors to this research. Mostnotably, Amy Newsam spent many hours searching and reviewing the critical thinking (CT)literature, developing stimulus materials and measures for the validation experiment,interviewing subject matter experts, and any number of assorted tasks that kept the projecttogether. We sincerely appreciate her efforts. Bill Campsey was also instrumental to the successof this project. We are grateful for his support of the Intermediate Level Education (ILE) team inwhich he helped them use the model of CT described in this report to develop curriculum forofficers at the Army Command and General Staff College.We would also like to thank John Lewis and the ILE team, particularly Jeri Gregory, fortheir early recognition of the value of our concept of CT. We have learned a great deal aboutimplementing training in CT from them. We are grateful for their hard work, conscientiousness,and patience as they worked to make our ideas a reality in training.The validation of the model could not have been accomplished without the psychologystudents who served as participants. We thank them for their service. We are also indebted toMajor James Pietsch, who served as an invaluable liaison between Anacapa Sciences and theArmy officers we interviewed at Ft. Hood, Texas. We learned a great deal about critical thinkingand battle command from the officers of the 1st Cavalry Division. We greatly appreciate theirwillingness to share their extensive knowledge and insights with us.Finally, we would like to thank our friend and colleague, Jim Kornell, for his criticalreview of the model of CT we have developed. His insights have strengthened our work, forwhich we are sincerely grateful.iv

CRITICAL THINKING TRAINING FOR ARMY OFFICERSVOLUME TWO: A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKINGEXECUTIVE SUMMARYResearch Requirement:Advanced training in critical thinking (CT) is needed for adult populations in many fieldsof work. Not surprisingly, the United States military is at the forefront of the effort to promoteand improve thinking skills. Military leadership requires the application of high quality CT foreffective battle command. As quoted in the Army Field Manual 3-0 (Department of the Army,2001).[It is] essential that all leaders from subaltern to commanding generalfamiliarize themselves with the art of clear, logical thinking. It is morevaluable to be able to analyze one battle situation correctly, recognizeits decisive elements and devise a simple, workable solution for it, thanto memorize all the erudition ever written of war.(Infantry in Battle, 1939, p. 14)Clear, logical thinking, or critical thinking, then, is at the heart of leadership. This report isthe second of a series of three that describe a research program to systematically and rigorouslydevelop a web based training program in critical thinking for Army officers.Before effective CT training can be developed, training objectives must be developed andthe critical thinking skills (CTS) to train must be identified. Ideally, those objectives and CTSshould be derived from an empirically tested theoretical model of CT. However, no such modelof critical thinking existed prior to this research. This report discusses the results of acomprehensive review of the literature in critical thinking and model of critical thinking thatevolved from this review. This volume also provides the results of experiments to validate themodel and to investigate whether the model is applicable to Army battle command. The latterinvestigation also identified critical thinking skills that were important to and difficult to executein battle command. The model and identified CTS serve as the basis for the development of aprototype CT training program for Army Officers that is reported in Volume Three of this series.Procedure:The primary purpose of this portion of the CT research program was to develop a testablemodel of CT. In this report, such a model is presented. It is based on current conceptions of CToffered by the philosophy and education literatures and is grounded in current psychologicaltheories of reasoning and judgment.An experiment was conducted to test some of the central predictions made by the model.Twenty-six participants (5 males and 21 females), ranging in age from 20 to 51 years of age,took part in the experiment. Participants were asked to perform three different tasks related ninev

written paragraphs, each of which described a different research investigation. The taskinstructions asked the participant to either 1) understand, 2) make a judgment about, or 3) simplyidentify the general topic of the material presented. The type and amount of substantive contentof the paragraphs describing the nine research investigations was also varied.A second investigation was conducted to determine the degree to which the CT model thatwas developed could be applied to the kinds of problems faced by Army leaders. Eighteen Armyofficers stationed at Ft. Hood, Texas participated in the investigation. Participants completed asurvey that assessed their opinions and experiences concerning CT skills as applied to thedomain of battle command.Findings:The model that was developed incorporates many ideas about CT offered by leadingthinkers in philosophy and education. It embodies many of the CT skills and predisposingattitudes discussed in the CT literature. It also specifies the relationships among a variety ofvariables that previous researchers have discussed, such as the influence of experience andknowledge, and the relationship of CT to cognitive tasks (e.g., judgment and problem solving).The model, however, goes beyond the largely rational/analytic work conducted to date byproviding a framework in which CT can be empirically investigated as a cognitive process.The validation experiment that tested several predictions of the model yielded a number ofresults. The results did not support the prediction that high substance material increases thetendency to apply CT skills. Under some task conditions, low substance material actuallygenerated more CT than high substance material. The results, however, did support theprediction that the application of CT skills is more likely if the available information is degradedin some way, i.e., is conflicting, disordered, uncertain, etc. The prediction that CT is initiatedwhen an individual engages in certain types of tasks was supported. However, it appears thatsome tasks may not elicit CT equally. These results suggest that refinement of the model may beneeded with regard to task. The results of this experiment also failed to support the notion thatpredisposing individual difference factors affect the tendency to engage in CT skills. Themodel’s prediction concerning experience was supported. It appears that experience doesincrease the application of CT skills. The findings of the experiment were mixed regarding thehypothesis that CT skills are associated with a corresponding increase in negative affect.Negative affect was not directly related to CT in this experiment.The results of the second investigation, which tested whether the model could be applied toArmy battle command, indicated that the CT model largely captures the critical thinking skills,situational conditions, and predisposing factors important to Army battle command. All of theparticular instances of these three variables were regarded as, at least, sometimes important tobattle command. Data obtained at Fort Hood confirmed that several battle command tasks areparticularly problematic, i.e., officers reported that they had observed deficiencies related to CTin these battle-command tasks. The results of this investigation were used to identify a set of CTskills important and problematic to Army battle command.vi

Utilization and Dissemination of Findings:The model of CT generated a number of predictions that previously had not beenempirically tested. The model was sufficiently specified to permit falsification of many of itsassertions, which other models of CT in the literature had not provided. The presentinvestigation tested five of the model’s central predictions. As a result of the investigation wehave a clearer picture of the effect of judgment and understanding tasks on CT and the effect ofstimulus substance on CT. It is now clear that CT does not always generate negative affect andthat experience may well increase CT.Although the results of the validation experiment were mixed in their support of the model,the model has passed an important scientific criterion. It has generated testable hypotheses thathave produced empirical findings from which we have gained knowledge. Some of the findingspoint to places in the model that require greater specification or modification. Other findings areconsistent with the model’s predictions.These results also have practical implications for the design of information systems and foreducational and training programs that seek to increase the use of CT skills. Designers andteachers should be aware that people may not question highly substantive material any more thanlow substantive material. If CT is desired, inconsistent content might be highlighted byinformation systems. Similarly, if educational and training programs seek to encourage CT, onestrategy would be to sensitize students to inconsistent material.The CT skills identified in this research as problematic to Army battle command may beutilized for training and assessment purposes and to increase self-awareness. These skills havebeen integrated into the Army Command and General Staff College Intermediate LevelEducation (ILE) course materials. The original skills identified in the first phase of the presentresearch are now incorporated into each of the major blocks that are being taught in the ILECommon Core course. Also, training concepts discussed in Fischer, Spiker and Riedel (2008b)have been adopted to teach the skills in ILE.vii

viii

CRITICAL THINKING TRAINING FOR ARMY OFFICERSVOLUME TWO: A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKINGCONTENTSPageINTRODUCTION . 1Societal Interest in Critical Thinking . 1Critical Thinking in the Military . 2Purpose of the Research Program4REVIEW OF CRITICAL THINKING LITERATURE . 5What is CT? . 5What Skills are Involved in CT? . 14How is CT Operationalized/Measured? . 14What Role do Attitudes Play in CT?. 19What Roles do Stimuli and Context Play in CT? . 20Who are the Best Critical Thinkers? . 20Can CT be Trained? . 21What is Missing from the Empirical Research on CT? . 22A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKING . 23Overview . 23Core Tenets of the CT Model . 24The CT Model . 25Contextual Factors . 25Meta-Tasks . 26Predisposing Individual Difference Factors. 27Controlled Process and Critical Thinking Skills . 27Moderating Variables. 30Negative Affective Consequences . 30Measures of CT . 31VALIDATION OF THE CRITICAL THINKING MODEL. 33Method . 34Participants . 34Materials . 34Design . 36Procedure . 36Results 38Effect of Task and Stimulus Variables on Indicators of CT . 38Effect of CT on Affect . 43Effect of Moderating Variable (Level of Education) on CT. 43Relationship of Predisposing Factor (Need for Cognition) to CT . 46ix

CONTENTS (Continued)Discussion . 48Effect of Substance of Material on CT . 48Effect of Task on CT. 49Effect of Predisposing Factors on CT . 50Effect of Moderating Variables on CT . 50Effect of CT on Negative Affect . 51Summary and Conclusions . 52AN INVESTIGATION OF CT IN ARMY BATTLE COMMAND. 53Method . 53Participants . 53Survey Materials . 53Procedure . 54Results . 55CT Skills . 55Discussion . 59SELECTION OF HIGH-PAYOFF CT SKILLS FOR BATTLE COMMAND . 60CT SKILL TRAINING AT THE COMMAND AND GENERAL STAFF COLLEGE . 66Background . 66CT Skills Incorporated in Course Curriculum . 66CONCLUSION . 68REFERENCES . 69APPENDIX A: CT SKILLS EXTRACTED FROM LITERATURE . A-1APPENDIX B: PREDISPOSING FACTORS FOR CRITICAL THINKING . B-1APPENDIX C: INSTRUCTIONS USED TO MANIPULATE TASK IN VALIDATIONINVESTIGATION . C-1APPENDIX D: TWENTY-SEVEN PARAGRAPHS USED IN VALIDATIONINVESTIGATION . D-3x

CONTENTS (Continued)LIST OF TABLESTable 1. Sample of CT Definitions, Themes, and Source Disciplines Provided in theLiterature . 72. Number of Propositions per Topic and Substantive Content Type . 353. Sample Descriptive Statistics for NFC and Other Measures . 474. Rank, Duty Position, and Branch of Participating Officers . 545. Means and Distribution of Importance Ratings and Frequency of Problemsfor 13 Broad Classes of CT skills . 556. Distribution of Importance Ratings for 11 Situation Conditions Associatedwith CT . 577. Distribution of Importance Ratings for 9 Predisposing Attitudes Associatedwith CT . 588. Core CT Skills Selected for Training Implementation . 639. Relationship of Battle Command Tasks, CT Issues and Selected CTSs . 6410. Critical Thinking Skills Incorporated into the Original CGSC-ILE Core Course . 67LIST OF FIGURESFigure 1.2.3.4.Process model of critical thinking . 26Mean response time as a function of substance of stimulus material . 39Mean reported effort as a function of task and substance of stimulus material . 40Mean number of questions of belief as a function of task and substance ofstimulus material . 425. Mean number of questions of belief as a function of substance ofstimulus material and educational level . 446. Mean number of questions of belief as a function of task and educational level. 447a. Mean number of questions of belief asked by undergraduates as a function oftask and substance of stimulus material . 457b. Mean number of questions of belief asked by graduate students as a function oftask and substance of stimulus material . 458. Mean number of checks on thinking as a function of task and education level . 469. Ratings of importance (x) by reported problems (y) for 13 broad classes ofCT skills . 61xi

CRITICAL THINKING TRAINING FOR ARMY OFFICERSVOLUME TWO: A MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKINGINTRODUCTIONSocietal Interest in Critical ThinkingInterest in promoting critical thinking (CT) skills has increased over the past 20 years in avariety of diverse applications such as public education, military leadership, nursing, technicalvocations, and corporate business (e.g., Cohen, Adelman, Tolcott, Bresnick, & Marvin, 1994;Fallesen, Michel, Lussier, Pounds, 1996; Miller & Malcolm, 1990; National Education GoalsPanel, 1991; Tucker, 1996). For example, the development of CT skills in students who attendpublic educational institutions has become a central component of the United States’ NationalEducation goals (National Education Goals Panel, 1991). In the military, research on leadershiphas identified the need for training programs that promote better thinking to improve battlecommand decision-making, and at least one course has been developed and implemented(Fallesen, Michel, Lussier, & Pounds, 1996). In the public and business sectors, the NationalLeague for Nursing has required the demonstration of CT in graduates of nursing educationprograms (Facione, 1995), DeVry vocational programs now assess the thinking skills of theirstudents (Tucker, 1996), and many US corporations now provide employees with training inthinking skills (Tucker, 1996). In short, increasing CT skills in United States citizens hasbecome a significant goal within government, public, corporate, and military arenas.Feeding and supporting this increased interest; CT has also become a recognized constructin philosophy, education, and, to a lesser degree, psychology. Unfortunately, authors varysubstantially in their stated and operational definitions of CT, and the field is highly fragmented.Yet, despite its messy conceptual state, CT continues to be an issue of concern for a diversegroup of interests.There are at least two reasons why this is so. First, society is experiencing an increasedneed for intellectual skills due to demands created by developing technologies. In the past 20years, our economic and social systems have become dependent on complex technologies, withinformation becoming either a primary or intermediate product that must be processed to servedecision-making. Humans must critically think about and mentally manipulate information tomake effective decisions. When information is incomplete, uncertain, or unreliable, the ability toevaluate its quality becomes paramount for competent decision-making. Thus, CT is a skillnecessary for the manipulation of information, especially when the information is degraded. Inthe future, greater reliance on information will necessitate greater reliance on CT skills. Allareas of society are experiencing increases in available information and a corresponding greaterdemand on intellectual processing. In some domains, however, technological development hasincreased the available information to such a high degree that job demands may soon exceedcurrent skill levels.Secondly, leaders in several domains (e.g., nursing, business, and military operations) haverecognized the need to improve the CT of personnel within their areas (e.g., Hawley, 1998).1

This concern is exacerbated by the decline in mid-level positions, with the attendant necessity forinformation-based decisions to be made by persons having less experience within manyorganizations. Moreover, it is not surprising that CT skills may be substandard becauseeducation in nearly all arenas has traditionally focused on the accumulation of contentknowledge, often neglecting to teach the reasoning skills that process such knowledge. Tucker(1996, p.5) remarks that “while content knowledge is a crucial part of this value creation, criticalthinking skills are the adjudicative engines that drive everything from boardroom strategic decisions to the creative responses of a software company's help desk.” In short, education andtraining may not have kept up with changes in skill demand.Critical Thinking in the MilitaryIt is not surprising that the United States military is at the forefront of the effort to promoteand improve thinking skills. Military leadership demands the application of high quality CT foreffective battle command, where battle command applies “to the leadership element of combatpower.Commanders visualize the operation, describe it in terms of intent and gui

critical thinking skills for Army officers, and developed and evaluated the training course. This volume describes the results of a literature review on critical thinking, a model of critical thinking that forms the theoretical basis for the training, and investigations that were conducted to validate the model.

Related Documents:

Bruksanvisning för bilstereo . Bruksanvisning for bilstereo . Instrukcja obsługi samochodowego odtwarzacza stereo . Operating Instructions for Car Stereo . 610-104 . SV . Bruksanvisning i original

Critical Thinking Skills vs. Critical Thinking Disposition Critical Thinking Skills are the cognitive processes that are involved in critical thinking Critical Thinking Disposition is the attitudes, habits of mind or internal motivations that help us use critical thinking skills.

2.2 Application of Critical Thinking in Nursing Practice 2.3 Traits of the Critical Thinker 2.4 Pitfalls in Critical Thinking 2.5 Critical Thinking Models 2.6 Critical Thinking Skills 2.6.1 Six Core Thinking Skills 2.6.2 Critical Thinking Skills in Nursing 2.6.3 Elements of Thoughts and the N

eric c. newman air force 2001-2009 george f. giehrl navy 1941-1945 f conrad f. wahl army 1952-1954 sidney albrecht . william c. westley jr. army 1954-1956 roland l. winters navy 1945-1946 michael a. skowronski army . joseph a. rajnisz army 1966-1971 james l. gsell army army army army army navy army navy air force army army

USG Critical Thinking Conference -Athens, GA* International Conference on Critical Thinking - Berkeley, CA* i2a Institute Critical Thinking Conference -Louisville, KY Spring Academy on Critical Thinking by The Foundation for Critical Thinking -Houston, TX Please coordinate with other conference attendees to

The Role of Critical Thinking in Problem Analysis Brian D. Egan, M.Sc., MBA, PMP Introduction Contrary to what the name implies, critical thinking is not thinking that is critical of others. It is “fundamental” or “vital” thinking. Critical thinking is thinking that drills down to the essence of a problem. It is introspective

10 tips och tricks för att lyckas med ert sap-projekt 20 SAPSANYTT 2/2015 De flesta projektledare känner säkert till Cobb’s paradox. Martin Cobb verkade som CIO för sekretariatet för Treasury Board of Canada 1995 då han ställde frågan

service i Norge och Finland drivs inom ramen för ett enskilt företag (NRK. 1 och Yleisradio), fin ns det i Sverige tre: Ett för tv (Sveriges Television , SVT ), ett för radio (Sveriges Radio , SR ) och ett för utbildnings program (Sveriges Utbildningsradio, UR, vilket till följd av sin begränsade storlek inte återfinns bland de 25 största